Skip to main content

Full text of "Paranthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal (Vol. 4 No. 3)"

See other formats


Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

Vol. 4 No. 3 (July 2013) 




/ 




ranthropology 

Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

Vol. 4 No. 3 (July 2013) 

I 1 

Board of Reviewers 

Dr. Fiona Bowie (Dept. Archaeology & Anthropology, University of Bristol) 
Dr. Iain R. Edgar (Dept. Anthropology, Durham University) 
Prof. David J. HufFord (Centre for Ethnography & Folklore, University of Pennsylvania) 
Prof. Charles D. Laughlin (Dept. Sociology & Anthropology, Carleton University) 
Dr. David Luke (Dept. Psychology & Counseling, University of Greenwich) 
Dr. James McClenon (Dept. Social Sciences, Elizabeth State University) 
Dr. Sean O'Callaghan (Department of Politics, Philosophy & Religion, University of Lancaster) 
Dr. Serena Roney-Dougal (Psi Research Centre, Glastonbury) 
Dr. William Rowlandson (Dept. Hispanic Studies, University of Kent) 
Dr. Mark A. Schroll (Institute for Consciousness Studies, Rhine Research Centre) 
Dr. Gregory Shushan (Ian Ramsay Centre for Science & Religion, University of Oxford) 
Dr. Angela Voss (EXESESO, University of Exeter) 
Dr. Lee Wilson (Dept. Archaeology & Anthropology, University of Cambridge) 
Dr. Michael Winkelman (School of Human Evolution & Social Change, Arizona State University) 
Prof. David E. Young (Dept. Anthropology, University of Alberta) 

Honorary Members of the Board 

Prof. Stephen Braude (Dept. Philosophy, University of Maryland) 

Paul Devereux (Royal College of Art) 
Prof. Charles F. Emmons (Dept. Sociology, Gettysburg College) 
Prof. Patric V Giesler (Dept. Anthropology, Gustavus Adolphus College) 
Prof. Ronald Hutton (Dept. History, University of Bristol) 
Prof. Stanley Krippner (Faculty of Psychology, Saybrook University) 

Dr. Edith Turner (Dept. Anthropology, University of Virginia) 
Dr. Robert Van de Castle (Dept. Psychiatry, University of Virginia) 

Editor 

Jack Hunter (Dept. Archaeology & Anthropology, University of Bristol) 

Cover Artwork 

Jack Hunter 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



2 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Introduction: 

Taking Experience Seriously 

Jack Hunter 



Consciousness is one of the great mysteries of 
contemporary science and philosophy (Nagel 
2012:35), and is currently undergoing some- 
thing of a resurgence of interest as a field of 
investigation (Zahavi 2005:3). Many commenta- 
tors have noted, however, that the bulk of re- 
search into the nature of consciousness pro- 
ceeds according to a fairly restricted idea of the 
types of approach that can be fruitfully applied 
to it (Jahn & Dunne 1997:204). Recently emerg- 
ing as chief amongst the dominant approaches 
is the neurophysiological approach, which at- 
tempts to understand consciousness as either 
identical with, or as an epiphenomenon of, 
physical brain function (Churchland 1982). 
This kind of reductionism, often referred to as 
'mind/brain identity theory' that is the idea 
that consciousness and brain-function are syn- 
onymous, is becoming increasingly popular in 
both the professional academic literature and 
the popular science literature (Searle 
1998:xii-xiii). Such reductionist accounts of 
consciousness have proliferated to the extent 
that philosopher and neurologist Raymond Tal- 
ks has coined the term 'neuromania' to refer to 
the belief that contemporary neuroscience 
proves that consciousness is identical with brain 
function and that free will is an illusion (Talks 
2012). The current debate over consciousness, 
and in particular the relation of consciousness 
to the brain (the mind/body problem), is there- 
fore torn over the question of whether con- 
sciousness can be reduced solely to the function- 
ing of the brain or whether it might be some- 
thing more than this. 

Whether consciousness can be reduced to 
brain function or not, however, the popular 
emphasis on quantitative, experimental, and 
neurophysiological approaches to the study of 
consciousness is not representative of the full 
spectrum of possible approaches. There are 
other means of investigation. Indeed, writing as long 
ago as the early Twentieth Century, psychologist Wil- 
liam James (1842-1910) stressed the fact that any 
model of the universe that fails to take into account 
the complexities of subjective experience will ulti- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



Contents 



Introduction: Taking Experience Seriously 
- Jack Hunter (3-8)- 

Experience and Studying the Paranormal 
- Charles F. Emmons & Penelope Emmons (9-13)- 

Lord of the Flies: 
The Phenomenology of a Possession 

- Peter Mark Adams (14-18) - 

Recognising the Voice of God 

- Tanya M. Luhrmann (19-20) - 

From Sleep Paralysis to Spiritual Experience: An Inter- 
view With David Hufford 

- John W. Morehead (21-28)- 

The Experiencing Brain 

- Charles D. Laughlin (29-34) - 

UFOs and Other Anomalous Phenomena: 
Connections, Beliefs and Perspectives 

- Jose Banuelos (35-40) - 

The Culture of War, "Afterlife Conscious Minds," & Mor- 
phogenetic Fields: The Past Soundscapes of an American 
Civil War Battlefield 

- John G. Sabol (41-47) - 

Processes of Experience 

- Donnalee Dox (48-53) - 

The Brain and Spiritual Experiences: 
Towards a Neuroscientific Hermeneutic 

- Andrew B. Newberg (54-62) - 



Ultra-Terrestrials and the UFO Phenomenon: 
A Response to Steven Mizrach 
- Jason Colavito (64-68) - 

Musings on Good, Evil and the Conquest of Mexico: An 
Interview With Graham Hancock 
- William Rowlandson (69-78) - 

[REVIEW] Through a Glass Darkly: 
Magic, Dreams & Prophecy in Ancient Egypt 
- Callum E. Cooper (79-80) - 



mately be doomed to incompleteness James 2004 
[1902]:335). Echoing this sentiment more recently, 
Thomas Nagel has written that '[t]he existence of 
consciousness seems to imply that the physical de- 
scription of the universe, in spite of its richness and 
explanatory power, is only part of the truth' (Nagel 

3 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



2012:35), the very existence of subjective experience 
implies that a purely physical explanation of con- 
sciousness is not possible. Consciousness is, after all, 
fundamentally entwined with experience (Blackmore 
2005:5), and it would seem counterintuitively detri- 
mental to attempt to divorce experience from the 
study of consciousness. Indeed the Oxford Diction- 
ary of Psychology characterizes consciousness specifi- 
cally as 'the experience of perceptions, thoughts, feel- 
ings, awareness of the external world, 
and.. .self-awareness' (Colman 2009:164). In the 
words of philosopher David Chalmers (1995) experi- 
ence itself is the 'hard problem of consciousness.' It 
would seem reasonable, therefore, to take subjective 
experience seriously, and to explore what experience 
itself might tell us about the nature of consciousness. 

Quantitative & Qualitative 
Approaches 

Qualitative methodologies, defined as 'forms of data 
collection and analysis which rely on understanding, 
with an emphasis on meaning' (Scott & Marshall 
2009:618), can provide a route towards investigating 
consciousness as experienced, and can reveal many 
aspects unobtainable through neurophysiological in- 
vestigation. For example, an fMRI scan could not 
express the redness of a red apple, or the blueness of 
a blue sky, let alone what it is like to experience 
pleasure, pain, love or hate. This is not to deny the 
relevance and importance of quantitative neuro- 
physiological research, rather it is a reminder that 
there is more in the way of richness and meaning to 
the experience of consciousness than is often pre- 
sented in neurophysiological accounts. Again, echo- 
ing William James, the richness and significance of 
experience are just as much a part of the universe as 
any physical object, and as such demand to be taken 
seriously. In order to examine subjective experience it 
is necessary to take a qualitative, phenomenological 
approach. 

The phenomenological method was first devel- 
oped in a systematic way by the German philosopher 
Edmund Husserl (1859-1938) as a means to investi- 
gate the 'structures of consciousness as experienced 
from the first-person point of view' In order to inves- 
tigate first-person experience in a meaningful way, 
Husserl developed the notion of epoche, a process of 
observation whereby all assumptions about a phe- 
nomenon are 'bracketed out' in order to understand 
it as it is experienced (Ashworth 1996:2), as 'pure 
consciousness,' without a priori conclusions about the 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



ultimate nature of the experience, or biased interpre- 
tations of it (Heath 2000:56). Robert Sharf, for in- 
stance, writes of the aim of phenomenological brack- 
eting in the study of religion: 

If we can bracket out our own presuppositions, 
temper our ingrained sense of cultural superi- 
ority, and resist the temptation to evaluate the 
truth claims of foreign traditions, we find that 
their experience of the world possesses its own 
rationality, its own coherence, its own truth 
(Sharf 2000:268) 

The phenomenological approach, therefore, aims to 
understand experience (or religion, culture, love, the 
paranormal, and so on) as experienced and under- 
stood by the experiencer, in its own terms. This is the 
qualitative nature of consciousness, what it feels like 
to experience consciousness. 

Hillary S. Webb has used the analogy of 'clock 
systems' and 'cloud systems,' first employed by the 
philosopher of science Karl Popper, to illustrate the 
different aspects of consciousness illuminated by 
quantitative and qualitative approaches respectively. 
Quantitative approaches focus on the 'clock systems' 
of consciousness, which provide 'insight into, and 
information about, the physiological and behavioral 
implications of consciousness,' factors that can be 
recorded and analysed using the standard methods of 
experimental science. Such research is useful in dem- 
onstrating the physiological correlates of conscious- 
ness, but ultimately cannot provide insight into the 
lived experience of consciousness. Research on the 
'cloud systems,' referring to those 'aspects of con- 
sciousness that are unpredictable and free flowing,' 
however, can begin to fill in the gaps left in our un- 
derstanding by the quantitative methods (Velmans 
2007a:724; Webb 2012:7). Qualitative data begin to 
fill the gaps left in the neurophysiological account. 
Without qualitative descriptions of conscious experi- 
ences the physiological description of brain states will 
forever remain incomplete. 



The Explanatory Gap 

The neurophysiological (quantitative) and phenome- 
nological (qualitative) accounts of consciousness are 
two sides of the same phenomenon. Firsthand, sub- 
jective conscious experiences are undoubtedly corre- 

4 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



lated with neurophysiological brain activity (Rees et 
al. 2002), and yet a so-called 'explanatory gap' per- 
sists because it is not yet clear how physical brain ac- 
tivity can be associated with subjective experience. In 
his famous article 'What is it Like to Be a Bat?' 
(1974), Thomas Nagel argues that: 

...the subjective character of experience... is not 
captured by any of the familiar, recently de- 
vised reductive analyses of the mental, for all 
of them are logically compatible with its ab- 
sence (1974:436) 

According to the dominant materialist view, physical 
matter is essentially inert, possessing no form of con- 
sciousness, which, naturally, is incompatible with the 
phenomenon of conscious experience. This problem 
is, therefore, a deep one, and runs at the core of the 
debate over consciousness: how can physical matter 
(such as the stuff from which we are made) have sub- 
jective experience? 

Max Velmans (2007b) recognises two distinct ap- 
proaches to the issue of the relationship between 
matter and subjective consciousness, which he labels 
discontinuity and continuity theories. Discontinuity 
theories essentially take the physical materialist ap- 
proach and suggest that consciousness emerged 
through the evolution of sufficiently complex biologi- 
cal systems (nervous systems and brains), and conse- 
quently is only found in sufficiently complex organ- 
isms, hence it is discontinuous in the universe - occur- 
ring only where complex organisms are found. Of 
course, this still leaves open the question of how and 
why matter, once it reaches a sufficiently complex 
state of organisation, becomes conscious. The alter- 
native view, which Velmans feels to be the most par- 
simonious, is the continuity model, according to 
which consciousness is a fundamental property of 
matter itself. This is a perspective that might be 
termed panpsychism. Velmans writes that according 
to this view: 

...all forms of matter have an associated form 
of consciousness, although in complex life 
forms such as ourselves, much of this con- 
sciousness is inhibited. In the cosmic explosion 
that gave birth to the universe, consciousness 
co-emerged with matter and co-evolves with it. 
As matter became more differentiated and de- 
veloped in complexity consciousness became 
correspondingly differentiated and 
complex.. .Its emergence, with the birth of the 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



universe, is neither more nor less mysterious 
than the emergence of matter, energy, space 
and time (Velmans 2007b:279) 

Currentiy the explanatory gap that exists between the 
physical structure and functioning of our brains and 
the subjective nature of our conscious experiences 
remains open, though there are models that attempt 
to close it. Only time will tell which model will prove 
to be correct (if indeed any current model is correct). 
For the time being, however, research must continue, 
not just into the physiological structure of the brain 
but also into the nature of subjective experience in all 
of its varied forms, in the hope that such research 
might contribute to the solution of these long- 
standing problems. 

Taking Experience Seriously: 
What Are The Consequences? 

Taking experience seriously, and using it as a means 
to approach the nature of consciousness may present 
the researcher with novel aspects of consciousness 
that would otherwise go unnoticed. Indeed, there are 
many peculiar quirks of subjective experience that 
might point us towards unexpected facets of the na- 
ture of consciousness. For example, what might near- 
death and out-of-body experiences tell us about the 
nature of consciousness? What might the trance ex- 
periences of shamans and mediums, the visionary 
experiences of mystics, or paranormal experiences 
tell us about the nature of consciousness? What does 
the psychedelic experience tell us about conscious- 
ness? There are countiess such questions, and we will 
briefly explore a few of them over the next couple of 
pages. 

Near-Death Experiences (NDEs) 

Near-death experience (NDE) researchers Sam Par- 
nia and Peter Fenwick have argued that the NDE 
poses a significant challenge to the notion that con- 
sciousness and thought are 'produced by the interac- 
tion of large groups of neurones or neural networks' 
(2002:9 emphasis added). They write: 

...the fact that [experiences recalled during 
periods of severely compromised cerebral 
functioning and no electrical activity in the 
cerebral cortex and deeper brain structures] 
raises some questions regarding our current 
views on the nature of human consciousness 
(Parnia & Fenwick 2002:9) 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Parnia and Fenwick suggest that the NDE experience 
opens up the debate over the nature of consciousness 
to alternative theories of the relationship between 
consciousness and the brain. As examples of alterna- 
tive scientific models they list Roger Penrose and Stu- 
art Hameroff 's theory of consciousness as a quantum 
process within neuronal microtubules (Hameroff & 
Penrose 1996), Rupert Sheldrake's notion of con- 
sciousness as a 'morphic field' (Sheldrake 1987), and 
the dualist idea that 'mind or consciousness may ac- 
tually be a fundamental scientific entity in its own 
right irreducible to anything more basic' (Parnia & 
Fenwick 2002:9). Whether consciousness is any of 
these things or something else entirely, however, the 
important thing to note in the context of the theme 
of this anthology is that taking the phenomenology of 
the near-death experience seriously demands a recon- 
sideration of the dominant mind/brain identity the- 
ory of consciousness, rekindles debate, and opens up 
new avenues for scientific inquiry. 

Trance Experiences 

As an illustration of the kind of insights that can 
come from taking the experiences of trance mediums 
seriously we now turn to recent fascinating neuroi- 
maging research conducted by Julio Fernando Peres 
and colleagues (Peres et al. 2012). During the practice 
of automatic writing (psychography), mediums claim 
to enter into a trance state during which their physi- 
cal body comes under the influence of a discarnate 
entity, which then uses the medium's body to write 
out messages using a pen and paper. During the 
trance the medium experiences a state of dissociation 
whereby the physical movements of their body are no 
longer felt to be under their conscious control. The 
standard materialist scientific approach to such claims 
is dismissal, because, according to the dominant ma- 
terialist paradigm, mediumistic phenomena are im- 
possible, therefore automatic writing must be fraudu- 
lent. Nevertheless, Peres' research team did take the 
experiences of mediums seriously and used single 
photon emission computed tomography to scan the 
brain activity of ten automatic writers (five experi- 
enced, five less experienced), while in trance. The 
research findings have been summarised as follows: 

The researchers found that the experienced 
psychographers showed lower levels of activity 
in the left hippocampus (limbic system), right 
superior temporal gyrus, and the frontal lobe 
regions of the left anterior cingulate and right 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



precentral gyrus during psychography com- 
pared to their normal (non-trance) writing. 
The frontal lobe areas are associated with rea- 
soning, planning, generating language, move- 
ment, and problem solving, perhaps reflecting 
an absence of focus, self-awareness and con- 
sciousness during psychography, the research- 
ers hypothesize. Less expert psychographers 
showed just the opposite — increased levels of 
CBF in the same frontal areas during psycho- 
graphy compared to normal writing. The dif- 
ference was significant compared to the experi- 
enced mediums (Thomas Jefferson University 
2012). 

The implication here is that during the trance state of 
the experienced automatic writers, activity is reduced 
in the areas of the brain usually associated with rea- 
soning, planning, language, movement and problem 
solving, suggesting that the medium's dissociative ex- 
perience during trance is far from delusional or 
fraudulent. Furthermore, the researchers conducted 
an analysis of the complexity of the writing and 
found that, contrary to what would normally be ex- 
pected, the complexity increased as the activity in the 
areas of the brain usually associated with such com- 
plex behaviours was reduced. This raises the question 
of how, if the brain's functioning was reduced, such 
complex writing was possible. The spiritist interpreta- 
tion suggests that it was spirits doing to writing while 
the medium's consciousness was absent, and the data 
could indeed be read in this way. More cautiously, 
however, Andrew Newberg has suggested that this 
research 'reveals some exciting data to improve our 
understanding of the mind and its relationship with 
the brain' and calls for further research in this area 
(Thomas Jefferson University 2012). Again we see 
that taking experience seriously, in this case the 
trance experiences of mediums, instead of dismissing 
them as delusional or fraudulent, opens up new ave- 
nues for inquiry and provides tantalising insights into 
the relationship between consciousness and the body 
that might otherwise have gone unnoticed. 

Psychedelic Experiences 

Interestingly, a recent functional magnetic resonance 
imaging (fMRI) studies of the effects of psilocybin 
(the active compound found in magic mushrooms), 
has revealed similar patterns of deactivation of cer- 
tain brain regions while under the influence of the 
psychedelic compound. The study, conducted by 
Robin Carhart-Harris and colleagues (2011), found 

6 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



decreases of cerebral blood flow in the thalamus and 
anterior and postulate cingulate cortex after the ad- 
ministration of psilocybin to research participants. 
The researchers also found that the magnitude of the 
decrease in blood flow was correlated with the inten- 
sity of the subjective psychedelic experience, leading 
to the conclusion that the results 'strongly imply that 
the subjective effects of psychedelic drugs are caused 
by decreased activity and connectivity in the brain's 
key connector hubs, enabling a state of uncon- 
strained cognition' (Carh art-Harris et al. 2011:2138). 
The association of heightened subjective experience 
with decreased neurological activity certainly poses 
interesting questions about the link between con- 
sciousness and the brain. Indeed these findings, 
amongst others, have led some researchers to suggest 
a 'filter theory' of consciousness, as originally sug- 
gested by Henri Bergson (1859-1941), and borrowing 
from Aldous Huxley's (1894-1963) conception of the 
brain as a 'reducing valve' for consciousness (Luke & 
Friedman 2010; Luke 2012:99; Kastrup 2012; see 
also Carter 2012 for an overview of the filter/ 
transmission model). This position suggests that 
rather than producing conscious experience the brain 
acts as a receiver of consciousness, so that when, un- 
der certain circumstances (such as mediumistic trance 
states, or while under the influence of psychedelics), 
brain activity is decreased so conscious experience is 
increased, or expanded (Kripal 2011:). Once again, 
taking the psychedelic experience seriously has pro- 
vided surprising insights into potential models of 
mind/brain interaction. 

This Issue 

The papers contained within this issue take a variety 
of theoretical and methodological approaches to the 
study of conscious experience, but all are united in 
their attempt to take experience seriously as a valid 
subject for inquiry 

Rather than attempting to present a unified ap- 
proach, it is the editor's hope that the different per- 
spectives presented in this issue (from the mystical to 
the neurological), will provide the reader with 
thought provoking material that might inform them 
in the development of their own particular approach 
to this fascinating aspect of existence. 

References 

Ashworth, P. (1996). 'Presuppose Nothing! The Sus- 
pension of Assumptions In Phenomenological 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



Psychological Methodology.' Journal of Phenome- 
nological Psychology, Vol. 27, No. l,pp. 1-25. 

Blackmore, S. (2005). Consciousness: A Very Short Intro- 
duction. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 

Carhart-Harris, R.L., Erritzoe, D., Williams, T, 
Stone, J.M., Reed, L.J., Colasanti, A., Tyacke, 
R.J., Leech, R., Malizia, A.L., Murphy, K., 
Hobden, P, Evans, J., Feilding, A., Wise, R.G. & 
Nutt, D. (2011). Neural Correlates of the Psy- 
chedelic State as Determined by fMRI Studies 
With Psilocybin.' PJVAS, Vol. 109, No. 6, pp. 
2138-2143. 

Carter, C. (2012). 'Does Consciousness Depend on 
the Brain?' In D. Pinchbeck & K. Jordan (eds.) 
(2012). Exploring the Edge Realms of Consciousness: 
Liminal %pnes, Psychic Science, and the Hidden Dimen- 
sions of the Mind. Berkeley: Evolver Editions, (pp. 
331-343). 

Chalmers, D. (1995). 'Facing Up to the Problem of 
Consciousness.' Journal of Consciousness Studies, 
Vol. 2, No. 3, pp. 200-219. 

Churchland, PS. (1982). 'Mind-Brain Reduction: 
New Light From the Philosophy of Science.' 
JVeuroscience, Vol. 7, No. 5, pp. 1041-1047. 

Colman, A.M. (2009). Oxford Dictionary of Psychology. 
Oxford: Oxford University Press. 

Hameroff, S. & Penrose, R. (1996). 'Orchestrated Re- 
duction of Quantum Coherence in Brain Micro- 
tubules: A Model for Consciousness.' Mathematics 
and Computers in Simulation, Vol. 40, pp. 453-480. 

Heath, PR. (2000). 'The PK Zone: A Phenomenol- 
ogical Study' Journal of Parapsychology, Vol. 64, 
pp. 53-72. 

Jahn, R.G. & Dunne, B.J. (1997). 'Science of the Sub- 
jective.' Journal of Scientific Exploration, Vol. 11, 
No. 2, pp. 201-224. 

James, W. (2004 [1902]). The Varieties of Religious Expe- 
rience. New York: Barnes & Noble. 

Kastrup, B. (2012). A Paradigm-Breaking Hypothesis 
for Solving the Mind-Body Problem.' Paranthro- 

7 



Paranthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



pology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the 
Paranormal, Vol. 3, No. 3, pp. 4-12. 

Kripal, J.J. (2011). Authors of the Impossible: The Para- 
normal and the Sacred. Chicago: University of Chi- 
cago Press. 

Luke, D. P., & Friedman, H. (2010). 'The Speculated 
Neurochemistry of Psi and Associated Proc- 
esses.' In S. Krippner & H. Friedman (eds.), Mys- 
terious Minds: The Neurobiology of Psychics, Mediums 
and Other Extraordinary People. Westport, CT: 
Praeger. (pp. 163-185). 

Luke, D. P. (2012). 'Psychoactive Substances and 
Paranormal Phenomena: A Comprehensive Re- 
view.' International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 
Vol. 31, pp. 97-156. 

Metcalfe, D. (2012). 'On Anthropological Approaches 
to Anomalous Phenomena: Explorations in the 
Science of Magic and the Narrative Structure of 
Paranormal Experiences.' In D. Pinchbeck & K. 
Jordan (eds.) (2012). Exploring the Edge Realms of 
Consciousness. Berkeley: Evolver Editions. 

Nagel, T. (1974). 'What is it Like to be a Bat?' The 
Philosophical Review, Vol. 84, No. 4, pp. 435-450. 

Nagel, T. (2012). Mind and Cosmos: Why the Materialist 
Neo-Darwinian Conception of Nature is Almost Cer- 
tainly Wrong. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 

Parnia, S. & Fenwick, P. (2002). Near Death Experi- 
ences in Cardiac Arrest: Visions of a Dying 
Brain or Visions of a New Science of Con- 
sciousness.' Resuscitation, Vol. 52, pp. 5-11. 

Peres J.F, Moreira-Almeida A., Caixeta L., Leao F, 
Newberg A. (2012). 'Neuroimaging during 
Trance State: A Contribution to the Study of 
Dissociation.' PLoS ONE, Vol. 7, No. 1 1, pp. 1-9. 

Rees, C, Kreiman, C, & Koch, C. (2002). Neural 
Correlates of Consciousness in Humans.' Nature 
Reviews, Vol. 3, pp. 261-271. 

Scott, J. & Marshall, G. (2009). Oxford Dictionary of 
Sociology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 

Searle, J.R. (1998). The Mystery of Consciousness. Lon- 
don: Granta Books. 



Sharf, R.H. (2000). 'The Rhetoric of Experience and 
the Study of Religion.' Journal of Consciousness 
Studies, Vol. 7, No. 1 1-12, pp. 267-287. 

Sheldrake, R. (1987). 'Mind, Memory, and Archetype 
Morphic Resonance and the Collective Uncon- 
scious - Part I.' Psychological Perspectives, Vol. 18, 
No. 1, pp. 9-25. 

Tallis, R. (2012). Aping Mankind: Neuromania, Darwinitis 
and the Misrepresentation of Humanity. Durham: 
Acumen Publishing Ltd. 

Thomas Jefferson University (2012, November 16). 
'Brazilian Mediums Shed Light on Brain Activ- 
ity During a Trance State.' ScienceDaily. Re- 
trieved January 11, 2013, from: 
http://www.sciencedaily.eom/releases/2012/l 1 
/121 117184543.htm 

Velmans, M. (2007a). An Epistemology for the Study 
of Consciousness.' In M. Velmans & S. Schnei- 
der (eds.) (2007). The Blackwell Companion to Con- 
sciousness. New York: Blackwell. (pp. 711-725). 

Velmans, M. (2007b). 'The Co-Evolution of Matter 
and Consciousness.' Synthesis Philosophica, Vol. 22, 
No. 44, pp. 273-282. 

Webb, H.S. (2012). 'Clock System or Cloud System?: 
Applying Popper's Metaphor to the Study of 
Human Consciousness.' Paranthropology: Journal of 
Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal, Vol. 3, 
No. 4, pp. 4-12. 

Zahavi, D. (2005). Subjectivity and Selfhood: Investigating 
the First-Person Perspective. Cambridge, Massachu- 
setts: MIT Press. 

Biography 

Jack Hunter is a Ph.D. candidate in the Department of Archae- 
ology and Anthropology at the University of Bristol. His research 
takes the form of an ethnographic study of contemporary trance 
and physical mediumship in Bristol, focusing on themes of person- 
hood, performance, altered states of consciousness and anoma- 
lous experience. In 2010 he established 'Paranthropology: Journal 
of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal' as a means to 
promote an interdisciplinary dialogue on issues of the paranormal. 
In 2010 he was awarded the Eileen J. Garrett Scholarship by the 
Parapsychology Foundation, and in 2011 he received the Gertrude 
Schmeidler Award from the Parapsychological Association and a 
research grant from the Society for Psychical Research. He is the 
author of Why People Believe in Spirits, Gods and Magic (2012), 
an introduction to the anthropology of the supernatural. 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



8 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Experience and Studying the Paranormal 

Charles F. Emmons & Penelope Emmons 




There is a great deal of doubt in mainstream sci- 
ence about the appropriateness of scientific research 
on the 'paranormal,' ranging from healthy skepticism 
to ridicule. Even some (many? most?) scientists who 
dare to study the paranormal display at least a 
healthy degree of skepticism themselves. Some of this 
is no doubt a reaction to the attack from mainstream 
science. However, keep in mind that scientists in gen- 
eral, no matter how mainstream or anomalous their 
subject matter, have not only been trained in the 
methods of science, but have also been socialized 
mostly in a Western cultural context that privileges 
science as a way of knowing. Even the Western spirit 
mediums we studied (Emmons & Emmons 2003) 
tended to be skeptical of their own work, often look- 
ing for 'confirmations' that their readings were evi- 
dential instead of something they were just making 
up in their heads. 

Therefore, it often takes some kind of dramatic 
personal experience for a scientist to get past a mate- 
rialist mindset and to become open-minded enough 
and curious enough to look into the study of anoma- 
lies. My favorite account of such an experience is 
Elizabeth Lloyd Mayer's (2007:2-4) adventure with 
her daughter's harp. The expensive, handmade harp 
had been stolen at a theater in Oakland, California, 
where she played in a concert. Having failed to find it 
after extensive help from the police, and media, 
Mayer reluctantly agreed to a friend's suggestion that 
she contact a dowser (a practitioner who allegedly 
finds things, underground or elsewhere, by means of 
dowsing rods). 

Her friend directed Mayer to the president of the 
American Society of Dowsers, whom she then called 
on the phone. From Arkansas, the dowser paused 
briefly, then told her that the harp was still in Oak- 
land and asked her to send him a street map of the 
city. Two days after she sent the map, the dowser 
called her back and told her, 'It's in the second house 

on the right on D Street, just off 

L Avenue' (Mayer 2007:3). Mayer located 

the house, then gave the address to the police, who 
predictably told her that 'a tip' was not enough 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



grounds to get a search warrant. Besides, they said, 
surely the harp had been fenced out of the area by 
then. 

At this point Mayer put up flyers in a two-block 
area around the house, offering a reward for the 
harp. 'It was a crazy idea... [and] I was embarrassed 
enough about what I was doing to tell just a couple of 
close friends about it' (Mayer 2007:3). Three days 
later a man called saying that the harp described on 
the flyer he'd seen outside his house matched exactly 
a harp his next-door neighbor had recently acquired. 
After two weeks of 'a series of circuitous phone calls' 
it was agreed that she would meet a teenage boy in a 
store parking lot. Sure enough, it was her daughter's 
harp. 'Twenty-five minutes later, as I turned into my 
driveway, I had the thought, This changes 
everything..! had to face the fact that my notions of 
space, time, reality and the nature of the human 
mind were stunningly inadequate' (Mayer 2007:3-4). 

After that she began to delve into the literature 
on anomalies and started to share experiences with 
her psychology colleagues and others at the Univer- 
sity of California, Berkeley, and elsewhere. She died 
just after completing her book Extraordinary Know- 
ing: Science, Skepticism, and the Inexplicable Powers 
of the Human Mind (Mayer 2007), the source for the 
above account. 

Fortunately we have more than just 'anecdotal' 
evidence for scientists changing their values or inter- 
ests based on personal experiences (and not just on 
their knowledge of research findings). In a survey of 
elite scientists, McClenon (1984:162) found that 'be- 
lief in ESP is more closely related to personal experi- 
ence [with paranormal events] than to familiarity 
with the research literature on psi.' In other words, it 
may be that research is less convincing than personal 
experience when it comes to 'things that aren't sup- 
posed to happen' (deviant knowledge). 

This does not surprise me. In my study of 91 
UFO researchers (84 of whom had advanced de- 
grees, including 76 doctorates), the most important 
single reason they gave for wanting to (daring to) 
study UFOs was thinking that they had had a UFO 



9 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



experience themselves (Emmons 1997:48-54). Alto- 
gether 48% thought they had had an experience, and 
another 8% thought they might have. This contrasts 
with polls of the general population in which only 
between 5% and 14% thought they had seen a UFO. 

Although some of these ufologists kept their work 
secret, most of them had undergone risks to their 
careers by conducting UFO research. It often takes 
some kind of powerful personal curiosity to be willing 
to buck the social control system in academe and 
government (not so much in business). As one of the 
UFO researchers told me ('Dr. X,' Emmons 
1997:52-54), after he and his wife and at least one 
other witness had experienced a brightly-colored low- 
flying ferris-wheel-shaped object that drove them off 
the highway, he no longer doubted that UFOs ex- 
isted; he just had to find out what they were. 

Lest you think that an experience always carries 
the day ('seeing is believing'), I should point out that I 
had some amusing interviews with astronomers for 
my UFO study in which they told me that they did 
not believe in ESP or other anomalies, then pro- 
ceeded to relate to me their own strange experiences. 
On another occasion I watched a tape of a man de- 
scribing his disturbing nighttime visitation involving 
what he interpreted as a ghost, at the end of which 
he stated, And I don't even believe in ghosts.' 

Mayer (2007:108, 113) relates that Hal Puthoff, 
on the last day of the CIA-sponsored program in re- 
mote viewing that he and Russell Targ worked on at 
SRI, thought to himself, 'I can't be doing this. These 
data can't be real; it's simply not possible.' But the 
evidence was too strong. He said, 'The problem lay 
with my beliefs.' I don't want to make too much of 
this psychological issue, because I still think that the 
main issue is social organizational (the interests of the 
scientific establishment and of those who benefit 
from it), but Puthoff 's case is still interesting. It shows 
how being socialized to the dominant paradigm 
makes it difficult even for scientists who dare to do 
the research not to be super-skeptical. 

Even studying how personal experience impacts 
scientists' willingness to study anomalies is easier for 
the sociologist in me to accept when I think about my 
own 'experience with experience.' Here are a couple 
of examples (see also Emmons and Emmons 
2003:93-109). 

Before the age of 19 I never thought that I had 
experienced anything paranormal, until I took a psy- 
chology course run by Professor John Fleming at 
Gannon College. Although I was an atheist at the 
time, and felt sure that the universe could be ex- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



plained entirely by the normal laws of physics, I was 
astonished to hear fascinating accounts of research 
on ESP and PK. Instead of taking an 'it can't be; 
therefore it isn't' attitude, however, I thought, 'It 
shouldn't be, but it seems to be, so I'd better check it 
out.' 

I decided to try a study of my own, one in PK 
(mind over matter). In the following summer I rolled 
3 dice at a time for a total of over 200,000 up-faces, 
'trying for' a 5 on each one. The results were hits 
11/2 to 2 percent in excess of the expected value, 
with odds billions to one against this outcome for the 
size of the sample. Professor Fleming consulted with 
J.B. Rhine on my data sheets, who said they con- 
tained typical 'decline effects' (very cold streaks after 
very hot streaks). Fleming also had my dice tested 
(rolled in a machine) in a lab setting without me pre- 
sent, and the dice appeared slightly biased against 
fives, meaning that the odds against my results were 
even greater than expected. 

That hooked me for life, I think, but my first ac- 
tual sociological/anthropological study of the para- 
normal didn't come until about 18 years later, in my 
book Chinese Ghosts and ESP: A Study of Paranormal Be- 
liefs and Experiences (1982), in which I used social scien- 
tific techniques to compare ghost experiences, among 
other things, in Western and Eastern cultures. I found 
that apparition experiences were very much the same 
phenomenologically in spite of significant cultural 
differences in beliefs (Emmons 1982). For example, 
firsthand reports of apparition experiences in both 
cultures almost never occurred simultaneously with 
physical effects, in spite of strong beliefs in Chinese 
culture that ghosts often attack people physically. 

Although I have had many other personal expe- 
riences that have boosted my curiosity, probably the 
most significant set of experiences got me interested 
in the research on spirit mediums in the United States 
(Emmons & Emmons 2003). Most of these experi- 
ences connect to the death of my mother in 1993 
(Emmons & Emmons 2003:101-107). I got the im- 
pression that I was communicating with my mother 
after her death, at first hearing her voice in my left 
ear. I could have chalked it up to my imagination, 
except that there were many evidential aspects to the 
communication. For example, on several occasions it 
appeared that she would help me find lost objects, or 
warn me about little accidents that were about to 
happen if I didn't avoid them (like a bike u-turning 
right back toward me, which oddly happened twice 
within about two minutes, with different riders on 
different streets). The warning was 'watch out,' which 

10 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



I heard internally a few seconds before each bicycle 
event. It got to the point that it seemed to me to be 
unscientific not to see some significance in such un- 
usual occurrences. 

Fortunately I still retain the skepticism to consider 
other interpretations (like clairvoyance rather than 
spirit communication). Such experiences may or may 
not convince anybody else, but they have been 
enough to stimulate me to study paranormal issues 
(probably at a cost to my career). I also never want to 
lose my skeptical side. After all, my curiosity addic- 
tion is not satisfied by 'believing' (I don't believe in 
belief; I believe in evidence, which includes personal 
experience), and accepting things without adequate 
evidence would be like cheating myself, or cheating at 
solitaire. 

Another interesting experience took place while 
at college in 1964, although I thought strongly at the 
time that it was probably a hoax: a table-tipping 
demonstration (Emmons & Emmons 2003:137-138). 
At a cast party after our final performance, four of us 
sat around a card table with our hands on top (no 
thumbs underneath, it appeared). The table rose a 
good foot and a half before I dropped under the table 
to investigate. I could discover no tricks, although I 
suspected two people who had whispered something 
to each other over the table before we started. 

Stephen Braude, philosophy professor at Univer- 
sity of Maryland (Baltimore Campus), and a promi- 
nent writer in the field of paranormal research, also 
had a 'table-up seance' experience in graduate 
school. He told me that several factors made it seem 
genuine: it was his table, the participants were not 
'jokers,' and it was in daylight. The memory of this 
experience, which he thought needed confronting, 
stayed with him, but he waited until he was safely 
tenured as a professor before becoming involved in 
research on such matters. 

Robert Waggoner (2009:4-7), a researcher in the 
field of lucid dreaming, had his own experiences with 
lucid dreams, precognitive dreams, and visions of his 
'inner advisor' by ages 11 and 12. Then he read 
books by Carlos Castaneda as a teenager and contin- 
ued to have lucid dreams, learning to practice staying 
aware within such dreams, which is still a practical 
focus of his research today. 

Russell Targ, laser physicist and remote viewing 
researcher, told me about his childhood interest in 
trick magic, which led to his experiencing apparently 
real ESP while engaging in his performance tricks. 
His curiosity over his personal experience led him to 
build an ESP teaching machine involving a 4-choice 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



option, with the target selected by a random-number 
generator. People could learn from feedback, know- 
ing what it felt like when they were successful. 

By contrast some researchers have become inter- 
ested in anomalies without first having personal expe- 
riences to motivate them. Other strong motivators 
come from reading and from social influences from 
friends and family (some of whom may have had 
their own experiences). Reasons given by UFO re- 
searchers are similar (Emmons 1997:51). This also 
parallels the reasons for spirit mediums becoming 
socialized into their role (Emmons & Emmons 
2003:210-217). In other words, in spite of the sociali- 
zation process and social control system in main- 
stream science (and religion), there are other ways for 
people to become socialized to 'deviant' knowledge. 

For example, Dean Radin (1997:300), psi lab re- 
searcher at IONS (Institute of Noetic Sciences), 
writes about his curiosity stemming from reading sci- 
ence fiction stories, something Russell Targ did as 
well. Radin says that people in his family, including 
himself, did not have paranormal experiences when 
he was young. He never had a conversion experience 
and has been hooked on the data only (Mayer 
2007:226). 

Radin did tell us at a meeting of the Society for 
Scientific Exploration, however, that he tends to have 
precognitive dreams as an adult. Once he had a 
dream that he would be in a car accident the follow- 
ing day. Not wanting to be in a car accident,' he said, 
he decided to take a very circuitous route to work the 
next day, one that he did not take ordinarily, but then 
he was rear-ended. I couldn't help speculating on 
how a New Ager or Spiritualist might interpret such 
an experience. For example, maybe the Universe was 
having fun with him, Dean Radin the big psi re- 
searcher, who conducts lab tests for precognition. It 
raises paradoxical questions about such things as 
whether the future is predetermined and whether one 
could change it based on prior knowledge. 

Darlene Miller, Director of Programs at The 
Monroe Institute (TMI), told me about a blend of 
social influence, reading, and personal experiences in 
her background. Having been raised a fundamental- 
ist Christian, and switching to atheism in college, she 
was later introduced to ideas from TMI by business 
associates who had attended the institute. This, plus 
contact with The Course in Miracles material, 
changed her perspective on things. The same associ- 
ates led her to try reiki healing, with which she had a 
dramatic experience involving intense heat that took 
her pain away in ten minutes. After that she took the 

11 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Gateway experience from TMI and moved to TMI 
the following summer. 

Robin Wooffitt, head of the Anomalous Experi- 
ences Research Unit, a sociological research depart- 
ment at the University of York, England, had early 
reading influences, somewhat like Dean Radin. As a 
child he was interested in comics, pop novels, horror 
films and things generally related to the occult. These 
led him to the paranormal and the supernatural. Al- 
though he recalled no anomalous experiences of his 
own as a youth, and only a couple of things in the 
past few years that 'could be data,' he told me that 
nowadays he is primarily very skeptical, having been 
more open to such things as a child. 

Al Rauber and Garrett Husveth, two paranormal 
investigators in the United States, both said in a joint 
interview that their earliest influences came from 
reading Al read a book on ghosts as a sophomore in 
high school, then read everything he could find on 
the paranormal, hauntings and ESP. Garrett said that 
he became interested in ghosts at age five or six, then 
read all that he could about parapsychology, EVP 
(electronic voice phenomenon), ghosts and hauntings. 
The two of them started working together in the late 
1 980s. Both of them seem more focused on investiga- 
tive methodology than on any personal experiences 
they might have had. 

Mark Nesbitt, historian and writer of the Ghosts 
of Gettysburg series (1991), told me that he had been 
interested in ghosts as a kid, and later as a park 
ranger in Gettysburg he would ask people if they had 
heard about ghosts on the battlefield or in the historic 
houses there. Of course the official position of the 
Park Service (and of the Visitor's Center in Gettys- 
burg, I might add, where I spotted nary a book about 
ghost experiences or ghost folklore), has been to deny 
or ignore ghost experiences, probably out of needing 
to appear 'respectable' I should think. However, 
Mark, wanting to be a writer, began to record the 
many experiences people reported to him, and in 
recent years he has had some experiences of his own. 

Back to academe, let me relate the background of 
four graduate students in the UK who were involved 
in studying the paranormal when I visited in 2008. 
Madeleine Castro, a PhD candidate at the University 
of York, England, said that she was curious about the 
unexplained from about age twelve, and she 'ques- 
tioned the God thing' Activities with other youths at 
renewal camps and around the campfire, including 
shared extraordinary experiences, contributed to her 
curiosity about anomalous experiences, which she 
now studies in a sociological frame. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



Sarah Metcalfe, also at the University of York, 
whose research involves a sociological and medical 
approach to spirit mediumship, was originally intro- 
duced to the subject by her best friend who was a 
spirit medium. Sarah also attended a Spiritualist 
Church 'for entertainment' rather than as a regular 
member. She started out believing in mediumship but 
is now agnostic about it, nevertheless retaining a re- 
search interest. 

Hannah Gilbert, another sociology graduate stu- 
dent at the University of York, told me that she had 
had no anomalous experiences as a child, but that she 
did have an interest in such things that was supported 
by her father, an academic psychologist. They even 
did some work together studying spiritual healing. 
Eventually she ended up doing sociological research 
on the subjective experiences of spirit mediums. 

Another graduate student, name omitted for 
confidentiality, was interested from an early age due 
to her grandmother who practiced mediumship, as- 
trology, and tarot-card reading. As an adult she 
helped run a community group that held workshops 
in these same subjects. Although her perspective has 
changed from her younger years, when she 'used to 
believe everything,' she ended up studying Internet 
communities involved in neopaganism and Wicca. 

Before concluding this chapter on how experi- 
ence spurs scientists into daring to research anoma- 
lous events, I should also point out that some people 
take the position that experience is actually more im- 
portant than science, at least in terms of convincing 
people to accept the paranormal as real. Tami Si- 
mon, in the editorial introduction to Measuring the 
Immeasurable: The Scientific Case for Spirituality 
(2008:ix-x), states, 'I am not a person who needs sci- 
ence or research to convince me of the benefits of 
spiritual practice.' However, Simon continues to ex- 
plain that science is useful for legitimating the use of 
spiritual practices in the work of medical profession- 
als, and for refining such practices. 

Paul Rademacher, director of The Monroe Insti- 
tute, although supportive of the use of science at 
TMI, said to me that we tend to think that something 
is real if we can prove it by science, but experience 
comes first. In his case, when he had a construction 
accident as a young man, he had the experience of 
breaking through the pain and into a state of peace, 
in which he was surrounded by a being of light. 
Later, while in the ministry, he heard a clear, precise 
voice go off in his head, telling him of a book he 
must read. Through such spiritual guidance he ended 
up at TMI. Skip Atwater, also at TMI, had numerous 

12 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



out-of-body experiences as a child. It was these expe- 
riences, plus his involvement with remote viewing in 
the military, that left him with little doubt about the 
reality of such phenomena. 

Finally I am reminded of a Spiritualist who said 
to me, after hearing about this research of mine, 
'Let's leave the scientists out of it.' As you might 
guess, I have no intention of doing that. However, as 
a (social) scientist, I am still very much interested in 
learning from people's subjective anomalous experi- 
ences. 

References 

Emmons, C.E (1982). Chinese Ghosts and ESP: A Study 
of Paranormal Beliefs and Experiences. Metuchen, 
New Jersey: The Scarecrow Press, Inc. 

Emmons, C.E (1997). At the Threshold: UFOs, Science 
and the New Age. Mill Spring, North Carolina: 
Wild Flower Press. 

Emmons, C.E & Emmons, P. (2003). Guided by Spirit: 
A Journey into the Mind of the Medium. NY: Writers 
Club Press. 

Mayer, E.L. (2007). Extraordinary Knowing: Science, Skep- 
ticism and the Inexplicable Powers of the Human Mind. 
NY: Bantam Books. 

McClenon, J. (1984). Deviant Science: The Case of Para- 
psychology. Philadelphia: The U of Pennsylvania 
Press. 

Nesbitt, M. (1991). Ghosts of Gettysburg: Spirits, Appari- 
tions, and Haunted Places of the Battlefield. Gettys- 
burg, PA: Thomas Publications. 

Radin, D. (1997). The Conscious Universe: The Scientific 
Truth of Psychic Phenomena. NY: HarperOne. 

Simon, T. (ed.) (2008). Measuring the Unmeasurable: The 
Scientific Case for Spirituality. Boulder, CO: Sounds 
True. 

Waggoner, R. (2009). Lucid Dreaming: Gateway to the 
Inner Self. Needham, MA: Moment Point Press. 



Biographies 

Charles Emmons is a 

sociologist at Gettysburg 
College and author of 
books on spiritual and 
paranormal topics. His 
latest book, coauthored 
by his wife, Penelope 
Emmons, is Science and 
Spirit: Exploring the Lim- 
its of Consciousness 
(2012). They also col- 
laborated on Guided by 
Spirit: A Journey into the 
Mind of the Medium 
(2003). Other publica- 
tions by Charlie include 

Chinese Ghosts and ESP: A Study of Paranormal 
Beliefs and Experiences (1982), and At the Thresh- 
old: UFOs, Science and the New Age (1997). Pe- 
nelope Emmons is an ordained minister and 
medium. She has given spiritual counseling (read- 
ings) for more than twenty years. Penelope has a 
BS degree in Education and a Masters in Social 
Work from Temple University. She has a private 
counseling and coaching practice in Gettysburg, PA. 





http://www.scienceandnewage.com/science-and-s 
pirit-exploring-the-limits-of-consciousness/ 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



13 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Lord of the Flies: 
The Phenomenology of a Possession 

Peter Mark Adams 




There are few human phenomena that carry the 
complexity and ambiguity of possession: it chal- 
lenges the notion of a unified, immutable self of a 
facile distinction between "acting" and reality; of 
who or what is the source of one's actions; even of 
humans as single isolated entities. (Cardena 
1989) 

If the eye could see the demons that people the uni- 
verse, existence would be impossible. 
Talmud, Berakhot 6 

A comparison of the 'inner experiences' of people 
suffering from involuntary possession offers unique 
opportunities for cross-cultural investigation. By 'in- 
ner experiences' I mean those arising from states of 
"cognitive, empathetic engagement" (Bowie 2012) 
that typify healer-client relations, especially in the 
area of natural healing 1 . Coherence across accounts 
dealing with similar cases and derived from a range 
of healers and their clients establishes a pool of expe- 
riences that can then be interrogated. Nevertheless, 
'inner' accounts of possession will always seem fan- 
tastical, contrary to science and threatening to our 
notions of common-sense, personal identity and 
autonomy. And yet, it is only by exploring these sub- 
tler aspects of reality that we can make progress to- 
wards understanding the deeper significance of the 
phenomenon (as well as certain other anomalous ex- 
periences). From the perspective of practical experi- 
ence possession manifests in five well-defined ways: 

(1) Infestation (indirect externalization of an 
entity's presence). 



(2) Oppression (physical harassment by an en- 
tity). 

(3) Obsession (domination of a person's 
thought processes and behavior by an en- 
tity). 

(4) Possession (forceful displacement of a per- 
son's identity by an entity). 

(5) Subjugation (voluntary relinquishing of 
freewill to an entity). 

(Amorth 1999:77) 

Since full possession often involves total amnesia, the 
nearest that we can come to understanding the inner 
experience is through its lesser manifestation: obses- 
sion. What follows is the frank and disturbing inside 
account of such a case. To provide context I have 
framed it with my own experiences dealing with this 
client. The events took place outside the context of 
any religiously sanctioned exorcism and are therefore 
free of the totalizing worldviews of the various faiths. 

The client was a happily married father of a new 
born, a business and information technology consult- 
ant with a major transnational company. He contin- 
ued to maintain all of these multiple responsibilities 
throughout the course of his treatment, which lasted 
roughly eight months. He was also psychically gifted 
and therefore able to perceive the progress of his own 
case from a unique inner perspective. He had initially 
come to me to learn a range of energy-work tech- 
niques. As our work progressed he became increas- 
ingly aware of 'blockages' in his 'energy anatomy' I 
therefore suggested that we conduct cleansing work 
on these. As the cleansing progressed the client began 
to psychically 'see' 'black, grape-like attachments' 
around his lower legs and feet. Each time I cleansed 
these attachments a week or so later he reported that 
they had returned. In addition his feet now became 



1 The intention and desire to facilitate healing provides the 'navigation', for want of a better word, that powers the shifts in 
awareness necessary to 'read' the client's 'field' for relevant information. This is a reversed or goal governed process moving 
backwards from an intent to heal to the retrieval of the information that will facilitate it. This type of goal governed, retro- 
causal process is best described as teleological. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 14 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



swollen and purple. The medical doctors he went to 
could find no medical cause for his problems. 

From my perspective, the re-generation of the 
attachments was indicative of deeper problems and I 
decided to get a second opinion. Despite my urging 
him to consider how he may be contributing to his 
situation, up to this point he had been unwilling to 
share the crucial information that over a period of 
time he had been experiencing frequent erotic 
'dreams.' While asleep, or in a dream -like state, he 
was approached by what appeared to be a beautiful 
woman who engaged in intercourse with him result- 
ing in orgasm. Such recurrent 'dreams' are often as- 
sociated with psychic 'parasitism,' and with a class of 
entities traditionally called 'succubi' (though 'djinn' 
encompasses parallel ideas). Such entities would be 
difficult enough to deal with, but this case was about 
to take an even darker turn. The following account, 
in the client's own words, is a rare description of the 
actual experience of the course of his possession per- 
ceived psychically. 

"I had had strange sensations in my legs and 
feet. Whilst I was meditating all of the energy 
went to the soles of my feet, which felt as 
though they were burning in a fire. Each time I 
undertook energy work the burning sensation 
in my feet got worse. My feet became swollen 
and purple all over. I visited medical doctors 
who diagnosed me with swollen arteries, pre- 
scribed creams and told me not to walk on 
grass because I might be allergic to it. In short, 
they hadn't a clue what was going on. My 
awareness of my condition came about 
through a meditative state. It was extremely 
fluid, alive, colourful and yet painful at the 
same time. I was in a place like a museum. 
Standing in one of the rooms a mirror caught 
my eye. I stood in front of the mirror. Sud- 
denly I felt as though I have been hypnotized 
and fixed in place. I felt as though two arms 
were holding me. Then I heard chanting and 
saw that a tattoo was being carved on my 
legs. I screamed in pain and tried to stop what 
was going on. I tried to use protective symbols 
and energy to stop the ritual. Luckily I was 
pulled out of this nightmare and back to reality 
by my wife. I knew that I had to seek help from 
someone who could undo or remove what had 
been done to me. Luckily I was referred to a 
lady who worked with higher beings who was 
able to help me. I don't want to go into much 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



detail but I realized that this was a recurring 
event in many past lives. I had been ritually 
pledged and sacrificed to a group of entities 
who used me to fulfil their own purposes. It 
was the fragments of these rituals that I had 
'seen' so lucidly and re -lived during my medita- 
tion. The outcome was that I now realized that 
I had attachments all over my body and most 
of my energy was being sucked from me. I was 
able to see the entities, who were acting upon 
the orders of a higher being. They were very 
clever and cunning. They knew all my interests 
and weak points. The entity tricked me into 
opening myself to it by disguising itself as a 
beautiful woman and approaching me when I 
was at my most defenceless, during sleep. In a 
dream, or perhaps better to say, dream-like 
state, the entity tricked me into engaging in 
intercourse with it. I started going to the 
healer. During my treatments I saw that my 
legs were covered with what looked like black 
grapes. These were the larvae of the entity. 
From the energies and substance of our inter- 
course this entity bred new entities like itself. I 
can only describe these as insectoid or, more 
precisely, 'fly-like'. It was like a horror movie. 
When they were exorcised I saw thousands of 
these 'flies' being released and returning to 
their place of origin. Only our trust in God 
can help us through these times. Love of God 
and nothing else. I am sharing this information 
because I believe our relations with such enti- 
ties are more common than most people sus- 
pect. We must open our awareness. Forgive 
ourselves and ask for help. If there is energy 
that can be collected and used as a breeding 
ground, anyone can become a target. Our rela- 
tions with these entities can be understood by 
analogy with fungi. If you leave a place dark, 
damp and with no ventilation than you will 
attract fungi. Our bad habits and vices provide 
perfect breeding grounds for them. We need to 
get a grip on our animal desires, we are given 
an intellect to learn, synthesise information 
and determine how we are to go forward in 
our lives. We need the light of God, we need 
goodness and goodwill. We should stay away 
from the things that we know are bad. Do not 
tell yourself that it is harmless, that it is just 
'one more glass', 'just a web site' or 'just a 
dream'." 

15 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Cases like this are often associated with a large 
amount of diverse and seemingly unrelated detail. 
Throughout this experience my client claimed that he 
was 'seeing' classic UFO-like craft. However, their 
scale was so miniscule that they were able to pass in 
and out of his body. He claimed to have seen typical 
'grey' aliens in his house and to have discerned tube- 
like 'attachments' that connected him to distant des- 
tinations through which life-energy was being re- 
ceived and syphoned off. Bizarre as all of these de- 
tails are, and no matter how out of place they may 
seem in the context of possession, they are neverthe- 
less consistent with numerous independent accounts 
(Baldwin 1998). The description of the fly-like being, 
the extraction of energy and the manipulation of his 
awareness are also consistent with these accounts 2 . 
Finally, during the healing sessions with the specialist, 
through her intercession he saw snake like entities 
emerge from her and enter his body to accomplish 
the healing work. Once more, this is consistent with 
accounts of neo-shamanic healing when working 
with a class of healing serpent-like entities tradition- 
ally known as 'Nagas'. 

After eight months of intensive work with the 
specialist my client has been pronounced 'clear'. The 
possession itself and all of the accompanying phe- 
nomena (ETs, greys, larvae) have been cleared up. 

Jacques Vallee has proposed a model for the in- 
terpretation of anomalous phenomena that employs 
six simultaneous dimensions or 'layers of interpreta- 
tion' (Vallee 2003). Given this framework we can 
breakdown the various components of this account: 

(1) Physical: none 

(2) Anti-physical: Presence of 'Grey' aliens and 
miniscule UFO craft capable of moving 
through the body. 

(3) Psychological: Manipulation of the client's 
behavior to make him more physically re- 
sponsive during sleep. Drawing upon un- 
conscious images of idealized beauty to 
clothe the entity's appearance and stimulate 
sexual arousal. 



(4) Physiological: Continuing sexual predation. 
Swollen, discolored feet that defied medical 
explanation. 

(5) Psychic: Vision of a 'past-life memory' in- 
volving the ritual pledging of the client to 
the entities. Vision of the entity itself, its 
larvae. Vision of healing serpents. 

(6) Cultural: The information arising from this 
case can be interpreted from within a num- 
ber of different worldviews including: a) 
psychopathology (e.g. dissociative identity 
disorder (DID) / possession trance disorder 
(PTD)); b) mainstream religious beliefs (it's 
the work of Satan & his minions); c) neo- 
shamanic 'perspectivism' 3 (De Castro 
1998); d) UFO studies (interpreting posses- 
sion as a sub-set of the alien abduction 
phenomena). 

We can appraise the variety of world views based on 
their economy and generativity: a) Psychotherapy has 
no one agreed approach to or understanding of such 
anomalous experiences. One the one hand they are 
seen as pathological symptoms of dissociation, alter- 
natively as a result of temporary psychoses (e.g. Qi 
Gung Psychotic Reaction) or as a 'spiritual emer- 
gency' (Assagioli 1989). Based on their clinical expe- 
rience a number of therapists and psychiatrists have 
shifted towards a neo-shamanic interpretation of 
spirit/entity possession (Fiore 1987; Modi 1988; 
Sanderson 2003); b) The idea that it is the work of 
Satan and his minions commits us to too much (neo- 
Gnostic worldview, elaborate cosmologies and spiri- 
tual hierarchies) and 'overdetermines' sensemaking 
with respect to the available evidence; c) Neo- 
shamanic perspectivism, "according to which the 
world is inhabited by different sorts of subjects ... 
human and non-human, which apprehend reality 
from distinct points of view" (De Castro 1998) pro- 
vides the most economical and generative option. We 
can conceive such perspectivism as an exercise in 
'worldmaking' (Overing 1998), an extension of the 
world view of consensual reality to takes account of 
the experiences reported worldwide by energy healers 
and their clients. These include multiple additional 



2 "There is another form of dark being. These are not created but spawned by the higher demonic beings and have no spark 
of Light within. These dark thought-forms are robot-like entities who do the bidding of the dark ones" (Baldwin 1995, 
p.341). 

3 "humans see humans as humans, animals as animals and spirits (if they see them) as spirits; however animals (predators) 
and spirits see humans as animals (as prey) to the same extent that animals (as prey) see humans as spirits or as animals 
(predators)." de Castro 1998. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 16 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



subjectivities (gods, angels, spirits, deceased persons, 
other and higher dimensional beings) and a corre- 
sponding increase in the sources of agency affecting 
sentient life (spiritually and energetically as well as 
mentally, emotionally and physically) interacting via 
such distinctive roles as preserver/healer, 
shapeshifter/ predator and prey. 

The convergence of traditional folklore, cases of 
'demonic possession', 'satanic ritual', 'UFOs' and 
'alien abductions' have long been noted in the litera- 
ture (Keel 1970; Vallee 1969; Vallee 1979; Baldwin 
1998). The effects of these phenomena can be highly 
reminiscent of the threefold process of initiation the 
world over (Van Gennep 1960), and in particular of 
the experience of liminality (Turner 1987), something 
they share, along with a pronounced sexual element, 
with the alien abduction phenomena (Thompson 
1989). It is as though the phenomena themselves exist 
in the conceptual overlap between these otherwise 
diverse domains of discourse, taking on the imagery 
and themes associated with them. Struggling with 
such fluid, multi-level phenomena we are, so to speak, 
victims of our own conceptual and linguistic prisons. 

The consensual worldview tells us that what we 
see is all there is. But this narrowness derives from 
modernity's 'buffered self (Taylor 2007 p.27), the 
self-segregated 'disenchanted' self (ibid p. 3 1-3 2) 
emerging from a failure of empathic engagement. 
This experience stands in contrast to that of the 'po- 
rous self (ibid p. 38) that empathically (and psychi- 
cally) breaches the walls separating itself from a 
broader range experience. "The 
enchanted:disenchanted distinction offers possibilities 
for better describing the ways in which different cul- 
tures experience this porosity. ... in some societi- 
es — and arguably for some people in all societi- 
es — lived experience does include the presence of 
spirits, gods, etc., as well as the possibility of being 
possessed by them. These might be accurately de- 
scribed as 'enchanted' cultures / societies or peoples." 
(Smith 2012, p.62 Note 7). 

On a final note, my client's character changed 
completely through the course of these eight months. 
A completely different person emerged out of this 
encounter with the numinous. From an expansive, in 
your face, can-do presence emerged one graced with 
sensitivity, spirituality and insight. This is, perhaps, 
the outcome one would expect from having under- 
gone the perils of such an initiation. 



References 

Amorth, Fr. G. (1999). An Exorcist Tells his Story. San 
Francisco: Ignatius Press. 

Assagioli, R. (1989). Self Realization and Psychologi- 
cal Disturbances. In: Grof, S. & C. eds. (1989) 
Spiritual Emergencies: When Personal Transformation 
Becomes a Crisis. Los Angeles: Tarcher. 

Baldwin, W. (1995). Spirit Releasement Therapy: A Tech- 
nique Manual. Terra Alta: Headline Books, 1995. 

Baldwin, W. (1998). CE-VI: Close Encounters of the Pos- 
session Kind. Terra Alta: Headline Books, 1998. 

Bowie, F. (2012). Material and Immaterial Bodies: Ethno- 
graphic Reflections on a Trance Seance. Available at: 
http:/ /kcl. academia.edu/FionaBowie. [Accessed 
on: 10 May 2013]. 

Cardena, E. (1989). Varieties of Possession Experi- 
ence. Association for the Anthropological Study of Con- 
sciousness. Volume 5, Number 2-3. 

De Castro, EV (1998). Cosmological Deixis & Amer- 
indian Perspectivism. The Journal of the Royal An- 
thropological Institute. Vol. 4, No. 3 (Sep., 1998), pp. 
469-488. 

Fiore, E. (1987). The Unquiet Dead: A Psychologist Treats 
Spirit Possession. New York: Ballantine Books, 
1995. 

Hunter, J. (2010). Ethnographic Encounters with the 
Paranormal. Paranthropology. Vol. 1, No. 2 (Oct., 
2010). 

Keel, J. (1970). UFOs: Operation Trojan Horse. New 
York: Putnam, 1970. 

Modi, Dr. S. (1998). Remarkable Healings: A Psychiatrist 
Discovers Unsuspected Roots of Mental and Physical 
Illness. Newburyport: Hampton Roads Pub Co, 
1998. 

Overing, J. (1990). The Shaman as a Maker of 
Worlds: Nelson Goodman in the Amazon. Man, 
New Series, Vol. 25, No. 4. December. 1990, pp. 
602-619. 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



17 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Sanderson, Dr. A. (2003). The Case for Spirit Release. 
Royal College of Psychiatry, Spirituality Special 
Interest Group (SIG) Resources 2003. [Online]. 
Available from: 

http:/ / www.rcpsych.ac.uk/ workinpsychiatry/ sp 
ecialinterestgroups/ spirituality/ publicationsarchi 
ve.aspx. [Accessed: 6 May 2013]. 

Smith, K. (2012). From Dividual and Individual 
Selves to Porous Subjects. The Australian Journal of 
Anthropology. (2012) 23, 50-64. 

Taylor, C. (2007). A Secular Age. Harvard: The Belk- 
nap Press of Harvard University Press, 2007. 

Thompson, K. (1989). The UFO Encounter Experi- 
ence as a Crisis of Transformation. In: Grof, S. 
& C. eds. (1989) Spiritual Emergencies: When Per- 
sonal Transformation Becomes a Crisis. Los Angeles: 
Tarcher, 1989. 

Turner, E. (1992). Experiencing Ritual: A New Interpreta- 
tion of African Healing. Pennsylvania: University of 
Pennsylvania Press, 1992. 

Turner, VW. (1987). Betwixt and Between: The 
Liminal Period in Rites de Passage. In Mahdi, 
LC, Foster, S. & Littie, M. Betwixt and Between: 
Patterns of Masculine and Feminine Initiation. Chi- 
cago: Open Court, 1998. 

Vallee, J. (1969). Passport to Magonia: From Folklore to 
Flying Saucers. Washington: H. Regnery Co, 1969. 

Vallee, J. (1979). Messengers of Deception: UFO Contacts 
and Cults. Brisbane: Daily Grail Publishing, 2008. 

Vallee, JF. & Davis, EW. Incommensurability, Orthodoxy 
and the Physics of High Strangeness: A 6-layer Model 
for Anomalous Phenomena. Science, Religion and 
Consciousness Conference at the University 
Fernando Pessoa, Porto, Portugal, 24 October 
2 00 3. [Online]. Available from: 
http:/ / www.narcap.org/NewPages/ Associated_ 
Research.html. [Accessed: 10 May 2013]. 

Van Gennep, A. (1960). The Rites of Passage. Chicago: 
University of Chicago Press, 1961. 



Biography 



I o, 



< 



Peter Mark Adams is 

a BA Hons Philosophy 
graduate with a spe- 
cial research interest 
in altered states of 
consciousness, epis- 
temology and the phi- 
losophy of science. 
Peter is a professional 
energy worker and 
healer specializing in 
Rebirthing breathwork, 
energy psychology and 
mindfulness meditation. Peter is the author of "Al- 
tered States / Parallel Worlds' a book length essay 
to coincide with appearances at the "Brain to Con- 
sciousness Conference', Istanbul, May 2011. Peter 
has just finished a new book "The Healing Field: 
energy, consciousness and transformation' dealing 
with the broad range of anomalous experiences 
that occur during energy based healing. This book 
will be available from Summer, 2013. Peter's other 
essays are available at: 

www.petermarkadams.com. Peter can be reached 
at: petermarkadams@gmail.com. 



Journal of Exceptional 
Experiences & Psychology 

Inaugural issue now available 




www.exceptionalpsychology.com 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



18 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Recognising the Voice of God 

Tanya M. Luhrmann 




I know what it is like to hear God speak. I am not 
a Christian. I am not even sure what I mean, 
speaking for myself, by the word 'God.' But for ten 
years I have been doing anthropological research 
among the sort of evangelical Christians who ex- 
perience God as interacting with them. They be- 
lieve that prayer is a conversation in which they 
talk to God and God talks back. They will say that 
God 'told' them to do something — to talk to the 
stranger next to them on the bus, or move to Los 
Angeles. To other Christians, this can seem in- 
comprehensible, and even dangerous. 

People often spoke to me about the first time 
they had recognized God's voice. Usually, this 
happened in prayer ministry. They realized that an 
apparently random thought or mental image was 
uncannily relevant to the person they were praying 
over, and they thought that God was telling them 
what the person they were praying for needed to 
hear. One woman remembered the first time this 
happened when she prayed for a stranger. 'I didn't 
know what to say. I was really scared. And then, I 
remember, I saw something. It wasn't a vivid pic- 
ture. It was more like my words described the pic- 
ture more than I saw clearly what the picture was. 
When I described it to the person I was praying 
for, he just started to cry. Then he explained why 
he was crying, and with that information, I was 
able to pray for him more. It was the most power- 
ful thing.' 

Once people began to feel confident that they 
heard God speak to them as they prayed for other 
people, they began to experience God speaking to 
them about their own lives. They would talk to 
God with their inner voice, about something that 
was vexing them, and they would wait for his re- 
sponse — some inner word or image that would 
give them guidance. Sometimes it came immedi- 
ately; sometimes it took time. They call this prac- 
tice 'listening' 

What I saw was that they were learning to pay 
attention to their inner world in a different way. 
The church taught that words from God should 
Vol. 4 No. 3 



feel as if they 'pop' into the mind, a spontaneous 
break from the flow of thought. 

Let us put to one side the question of whether 
God is really speaking, and examine the practice 
anthropologically. The first thing to notice is that 
the practice takes advantage of what we might call 
the 'texture' of mental experience. We have 
thoughts that are more startling and surprising 
than others; thoughts that seem a piece of the psy- 
chic river of awareness and thoughts that seem to 
come out of nowhere. These Christians treat these 
contours as significant. 

But they do more than attend to thought dif- 
ferently. The church teaches congregants to pay 
attention only to certain of these striking 
thoughts — to good thoughts, thoughts that are the 
kinds of things God should say. That is, those 
thoughts should be relevant, wise and loving. ('God 
does not tell you to hurt yourself,' people said.) You 
should feel good when you have them. When you 
hear God correctly, you should feel peace, and if 
you didn't feel peaceful, it wasn't God. 

Doing this changes you. One man explained to 
me how much his experience of God had altered 
since coming to the church. 'God's voice is like a 
fuzzy radio station, 95.2, 94.9, which needs more 
tuning. You're picking up the song, and it's not so 
clear sometimes. It's clearer to me now' That was 
why I say that I think I know what it is like to hear 
God speak. I worshipped with these charismatic 
evangelicals. I prayed with them. I read their 
books. I sought to pay attention to my inner world 
the way they did. As I did so, I began to have ex- 
periences like the ones they reported. I remember 
with clarity the first time it happened. I was trying 
to compose a note to someone — one of those 
complicated notes you need to send to someone 
you don't know well, when you want to be per- 
sonal but not forward. I fretted about the note off 
and on for a few days. Then suddenly the sen- 
tences just came to me. I didn't feel that I had cho- 
sen them. They came to me and I wrote them 
down and they were perfect. To some extent, the 

19 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



practice works. My ethnographic and experimen- 
tal work confirm this again and again. 

Religion demands of its followers that they 
understand reality to be different from the material 
world they live within — more fair, more good. It 
demands that they use their minds to present real- 
ity as different and as better. It is worth recogniz- 
ing that this is as much skill as belief, a knowing 
how (to borrow from the philosopher Gilbert Ryle) 
as a knowing that. The skill is probably at the 
heart of what makes psychotherapy work when it 
works, and probably what makes placebo effective. 
It's a different way of thinking about God than the 
science-religion wars suggest, and possibly less di- 
visive. 



Biography 

Tanya Marie Luhr- 
mann is the Watkins 
University Professor 
in the Stanford An- 
thropology Depart- 
ment. Her books in- 
clude Persuasions of 
the Witch's Craft, 
(Harvard, 1989); The 
Good Parsi (Harvard 
1996); Of Two Minds 
(Knopf 2000) and 

When God Talks Back (Knopf 2012). In general, her 
work focuses on the way that ideas held in the 
mind come to seem externally real to people, and 
the way that ideas about the mind affect mental 
experience. One of her recent project compares the 
experience of hearing distressing voices in India 
and in the United States. 




paranormal UFOs 

vampires psychology 

«ho* se r i o us ly^ngr 

£!r st ran 26 

consciousness mediums 



THE Paranormal 
Event of tb^ year 



A weekend of live experiments, 
research and some of the most 
respected speakers of our age 

6-8 September, University of Bath 

assap.ac.uk/SS 



fr-8*septfZ0i3 

University -t)f Bath 




Includes 

Gu L on Pla fair 

Enfield poltergeist investigator 

Lembit Opik 

Former MP and UFO speaker 

Prof Chris French 

Academic and scepitc 

Stephen Volk 

BAFTA winner, TVIfilm writer 

Sacha Claire Christie 

Experiencer of note 

Richard Freeman 

Professional cypto oologist 

Professor Chris Roe 

Parapsychologist 

Lionel Fanthorpe 

The Reverend himself! 

David Farrant 

Highgate vampire investigator 

And man more 
top names 



Seriousl Strange 

Return of the Biggest 
Paranormal Event of its 
kind in a Generation 

Live experiments and research - 
watch or take part! 

■f Beautiful, historic cit setting 

Seriousl Strange is the ke note paranormal 
event of the ear - not to be missed! 

w: assap.ac.uk/SS 



20 of the most respected speakers of 
our times on a vast arra of subjects 

A big part and disco 

Cheap rooms on campus 

One or two special surprises! 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



20 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



From Sleep Paralysis to Spiritual Experience 
An Interview With David Hufford 

John W. Morehead 




David Hufford has been pursuing research on the 
"Old Hag" sleep paralysis phenomenon for quite 
some time. Perhaps his best-known work on this is 
The Terror That comes in the Night: An Experience- Centered 
Study of Supernatural Assault Traditions (Philadelphia, 
University of Pennsylvania Press; 2 nd ed, 1989). Huf- 
ford joined the faculty of the Penn State College of 
Medicine in 1974 in the Department of Behavioral 
Science. When he retired in 2007 he held a Univer- 
sity Professorship and was chair of the Department of 
Humanities with appointments in Departments of 
Neural and Behavioral Science, Family & Commu- 
nity Medicine, and Psychiatry. Hufford is now Uni- 
versity Professor Emeritus at Penn State College of 
Medicine, Senior Fellow for Spirituality at the Sam- 
ueli Institute, and Adjunct Professor of Religious 
Studies at the University of Pennsylvania. Hufford is 
also a founding member of the Editorial Boards of 
Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing and Spirituality 
in Clinical Practice. 

John Morehead: David, thank you for your will- 
ingness to be a part of this interview. Your research 
on the sleep paralysis phenomenon is well known. 
How did you come to develop a personal interest in 
it, and how did your research on the "Old Hag" phe- 
nomenon in Newfoundland perhaps begin this proc- 
ess on an academic level? 

David Hufford: That, John, is a very good ques- 
tion. It goes to the very center of my professional in- 
terests, values and goals. In December of 1963 I was 
a college sophomore. One night I went to bed early 
in my off campus room. I had just completed the last 
of my final exams for the term, and I was tired. I 
went to bed about 6 o'clock, looking forward confi- 
dently to a long and uninterrupted night's sleep. In 
that I was mistaken. 

About 2 hours later I was awakened by the sound 
of my door being opened, and footsteps approached 
the bed. I was lying on my back and the door was 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



straight ahead of me. But the room was pitch dark, so 
when I opened my eyes I could see nothing. I as- 
sumed a friend was coming to see if I wanted to go to 
dinner. I tried to turn on the light beside my bed, but 
I couldn't move or speak. I was paralyzed. The foot- 
steps came to the side of my bed, and I felt the mat- 
tress go down as someone climbed onto the bed, knelt 
on my chest and began to strangle me. I really 
thought that I was dying. But far worse than the feel- 
ings of being strangled were the sensations associated 
with what was on top of me. I had an overwhelming 
impression of evil, and my reaction was primarily re- 
vulsion. Whatever was on my chest was not just de- 
structive; it was absolutely disgusting. I shrank from 
it. 

I struggled to move, but it was as though I could 
not find the "controls." Somehow I no longer knew 
how to move. And then I did move, I think my hand 
was first, and then my whole body. I leaped out of 
bed, heart racing, and turned on the light to find the 
room empty. I ran downstairs where my landlord sat 
watching TV "Did someone go past you just now?" 
He looked at me like I was crazy and said "no." 

I never forgot that experience, but I told no one 
about it for the next eight years. There was no ques- 
tion of interpreting this experience, locating it within 
my cultural frame. There was no place for it there. 
Dream? I knew, absolutely knew, I had been awake. 
Hallucination? I was sure that I was not crazy, but I 
also knew this would not be convincing to others. 
The insane are, according to stereotype, the last to 
know. So the experience just hung there, uncon- 
nected. Disturbing. 

In 1970 I traveled to Newfoundland, Canada, to 
do my doctoral dissertation fieldwork. I went to study 
supernatural belief. I was probably influenced by my 
bizarre experience, but I was also responding to a 
larger interest. In graduate school at the University of 
Pennsylvania I had been taught that supernatural 
beliefs are non-rational, unsupportable by proper rea- 
soning, and that they are non-empirical, lacking any 
sound observational basis. This seemed too sweeping 

21 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



and a bit arrogant, so in my research I proposed to 
ask whether traditional beliefs might have some ra- 
tional and empirical elements. I went to Newfound- 
land because it is isolated and has a strong traditional 
culture, the kind of place where I had been taught 
one might find remnants of pre -modern belief. It 
proved to be a good choice. 

While doing my research I taught at Newfound- 
land's Memorial University in the Folklore Depart- 
ment and worked in the department's extensive ar- 
chive. Almost immediately I found the Old Hag, al- 
though at the moment it happened it felt more as if 
the Old Hag had found me — again. When you "have 
the Old Hag," Newfoundlanders said, you awoke to 
find yourself unable to move. The hag, a terrifying 
something, could be heard coming, footsteps ap- 
proaching your room. The hag would enter your 
room and press you, crushing the breath out of you. 
If the experience is not interrupted they said it could 
end in death. 

The Old Hag presented me with a dilemma. I 
had been taught that stories about supernatural expe- 
riences confirming local traditions are produced by 
cultural influences, what I have called The Cultural 
Source Hypothesis (CSH). But the Old hag had come 
into my room in 1963 out of a cultural void. Tradi- 
tion says, "We believe this because it has happened to 
us." Modern scholarship reverses this and says, "You 
think this happens because you believe it." My di- 
lemma: I could explain the Old Hag based on cul- 
tural processes that confirm local cultural tradi- 
tions — although I knew that my own prior experience 
flatly contradicted such explanations. Or I could de- 
velop an entirely new kind of explanation. 

This all amounted to a stunning discovery. I now 
knew something about the Old Hag tradition that no 
one else seemed to know. But I was in no better posi- 
tion to proclaim this publicly than I had been to talk 
about my experience in 1963. I did not want to say, 
"Hey, that happened to me too! So that tells us that.... 
Trust me on this!" Personal experience lends authen- 
ticity and expertise to scholarly work, when the expe- 
rience is granted to be real — experiences of illness, of 
being in prison, of being an artist, of gender, of race, 
of all sorts of recognized categories of experience. 
But contested experiences have the opposite effect; 
they are seen as pure bias, "Oh, he's a believer (and 
therefore not be trusted)." If I were to place my expe- 
rience and my Newfoundland findings within a sensi- 
ble cultural frame, it would have to be a frame partly 
of my own making. In that way the personal became 
professional, academic. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



John Morehead: How has your academic discipline 
of folklore studies been important in your under- 
standing of the phenomenon? And what do you think 
about the use of other disciplines, like anthropology, 
to help us understand it? 

David Hufford: I entered the discipline of folklore 
in the mid-1960s because it included "folk belief" as 
a recognized topic for research, and because it had a 
populist orientation. In general it showed great re- 
spect for the views of ordinary people. In art, archi- 
tecture, oral literature, agricultural methods, etcetera, 
folklore stood up for the worth of ordinary culture. 
But I quickly discovered in graduate school that un- 
like other cultural genres, folk belief and respect for 
the knowledge claims of ordinary people occupied 
structurally antithetical positions in the discipline. 
Although folk music scholars did not judge by the 
standards of classical composition, folk belief schol- 
ars did, in fact, judge "superstition" by its conformity 
to current scientific opinion. Considering that most 
folk beliefs had never been subjected to systematic 
scientific research this seemed pure, unjustified eth- 
nocentrism. My anthropology training presented a 
related but more modern problem. 

The Boasian turn from blatant ethnocentrism to 
a sort of protective hermeneuticism offered the kind 
of patronizing acceptance that a psychotherapist of- 
fers to a psychotic patient: I believe that your halluci- 
nations are real to you. Finding internal consistency 
and rejecting evaluative comparisons to external 
knowledge, folk belief was accorded "its own logic." 
This fit well with the 20 th century scholarly resistance 
to comparative method. The post-modern turn re- 
jected not only scientific reduction but also all other 
efforts to obtain objective knowledge through com- 
parison. Scientific positivism reduced all sorts of folk 
beliefs to cultural fictions. Folklore and anthropology, 
in fact the social sciences and the humanities in gen- 
eral, were of little assistance as I wrestled with the 
"Old Hag." In fact, with regard to "folk belief" I 
came to see these academic disciplines as functioning 
to protect modernity from being challenged by the 
knowledge of other cultures and times. Ironically, this 
is similar to the function of positivism, but it offers 
the advantage of apparently respecting the knowl- 
edge claims it rejects. 

John Morehead: Can you summarize the basic 
elements that define the sleep paralysis phenomenon? 

22 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



David Hufford: Sleep paralysis (SP) refers to the 
loss of voluntary movement either during the period 
just before sleep (hypnagogic stage or sleep onset) or 
just after (hypnopompic stage). The paralysis is pro- 
duced by a cholinergic mechanism in the reticular 
activating system in the brain stem that functions to 
prevent the sleeper from physically carrying out ac- 
tions occurring in dreams. This atonia-producing 
mechanism is a normal feature of rapid eye- 
movement sleep. In SP this mechanism intrudes into 
wakefulness. This might suggest that the "intruder" 
experience of SP is "just dreaming" while awake. 
The problem is this: dreams vary greatly from subject 
to subject and over time, and their content tends to 
reflect inputs from the dreamer's waking life, together 
with aspects of the sensed environment (e.g., in a hot 
room one may dream of a tropical environment). 
The "Old Hag" is very different. It is as if dreamers 
all of over the world and throughout history report 
the same dream, and that repeated content does not re- 
quire the subject's prior knowledge! Furthermore the 
contents do not reflect the range of possible features 
that could arise from waking consciousness during 
REM sleep, rather being restricted to a very narrow 
spectrum; e.g., people do not experience the ceiling 
falling on them or terrorists entering their room, ei- 
ther of which would conform to the pressure and 
immobility of the experience. 

John Morehead: In the 1980s you wrote The Terror 
That Comes in the Night: An Experience- Centered Study of 
Supernatural Assault Traditions. What types of conclu- 
sions did you come to about the phenomenon at that 
time? 

David Hufford: My conclusions were data driven, 
and my data was especially rich, ranging from an- 
thropological and historical documentation to phe- 
nomenology to medical and neurophysiological find- 
ings, because I employed mixed methods, including 
ethnographic interviews, surveys, and literature re- 
view. The ethnographic interviewing was phenome- 
nologically oriented, aimed at developing a detailed 
description of the range of perceptual features of SP. 
These interviews began with open-ended questions 
such as, "Please tell me all that you recall about your 
experience." No questions probed for the features 
with which I was familiar; e.g., I never asked, "Was 
there a presence in the room with you?" My research 
design predicted that "the Old Hag" could be ex- 
plained by the cultural source hypothesis as cultural 
elaborations of SP (although my own experience had 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



already shown me that this was not possible), and 
asked whether objective findings conformed to that 
prediction. They did not. 

My interviews revealed a stable phenomenologi- 
cal pattern very similar to what I had experienced in 
college. The surveys showed that this pattern did not 
depend on cultural input or prior knowledge of any 
kind. The literature review documented reports con- 
sistent with SP in cultures all over the world and 
throughout history, although such reports had not 
previously been connected with SP The terms used 
for description in different traditions were obviously 
culturally determined, such as "Old Hag," the Mara 
(Tillhagen 1969) of Sweden, the da chor (Tobin & 
Friedman 1983), dab coj, poj ntxoog (Munger 1986), or 
dab tsog (Adler 1991) in Southeast Asia, the sitting 
ghost or bei Guai chaak (being pressed by a ghost) 
(Emmons 1 982) in China, kanashibari in Japan, and 
many more from around the world and throughout 
history refer to the same event characterized by pa- 
ralysis, the conviction of wakefulness before or 
emerging from sleep. These cultural terms were asso- 
ciated with a variety of other details such as soft 
shuffling footsteps and the shadow man' or misty 
presence, regardless of cultural context. A detailed 
review of modern scientific knowledge of SP found 
neither any awareness of this distinctive phenome- 
nological pattern, nor any mechanisms that would 
account for it. 

So, my conclusions in The Terror stemmed from 
the way that my research contradicted the Cultural 
Source Hypothesis as an explanation of "the Old 
Hag" and similar traditions. In its place I found that 
this phenomenon fit, instead, the Experiential Source 
Hypothesis: (1) many traditions of supernatural as- 
sault around the world refer the phenomenon known 
as sleep paralysis in modern sleep research, (2) scien- 
tific knowledge of SP lacks knowledge of its cross- 
culturally consistent phenomenology and has no ade- 
quate explanation for that pattern, (3) the cross- 
contextual perceptual patterning is what reason leads 
us to expect of accurate reports from independent 
witnesses, therefore (4) traditions of supernatural as- 
sault that contain the SP pattern are empirically 
based and rationally derived. 

John Morehead: Of course, your research contin- 
ued beyond the 1980s. How did this develop, and 
how did your understandings develop by 2005 when 
you wrote your essay "Sleep Paralysis as Spiritual Ex- 
perience" for the journal Transcultural Psychology? 

23 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



David Hufford: In 1974 I finished my Ph.D., re- 
turned from Newfoundland and accepted the posi- 
tion of Assistant Professor of Behavioral Science at 
Penn State's College of Medicine. I was offered this 
position based on the stance I developed in my doc- 
toral dissertation, Folklore Studies Applied to Health (Uni- 
versity of Pennsylvania 1974), which was focused on 
folk belief. I explored ways that the study of folk be- 
lief could serve medical research and care. Chapter 6 
was devoted to the Old Hag and SP. I saw two major 
connections to medicine: (1) belief is a major deter- 
minant of health behaviour (from patients' beliefs 
about etiology and treatment to doctors' beliefs about 
patients), and (2) the fact that in the 20 th century 
medicine, psychiatry in particular, had provided prac- 
tically all explanations for "folk belief" (meaning false 
belief traditionally supported), especially experiential 
claims in support of folk belief, through psychopa- 
thology (wish fulfilment, unconscious sexual forces, 
delusions, hallucinations, etc.). The journey I em- 
barked on in my Newfoundland research was per- 
fectly suited to the medical context, although in a 
somewhat perverse way. I accepted the appointment 
to work to improve medical care and diagnosis, but to 
do that I would have to directly address the harm 
done by medical misunderstandings. Ironically, folk- 
lore and anthropology (et al.) had been complicit in 
those misunderstandings. So, I went to medicine to 
subvert the received worldviews of modern intellec- 
tuals, in order to advance medical care. The Terror was 
a major part of that program. 

A central aspect of my subversive agenda was to 
pursue the extension of the Experiential Source Hy- 
pothesis beyond SP to other spiritual experiences. By 
spiritual I mean whatever refers to spirit, which in Eng- 
lish means the immaterial part of a living being. Part 
of my subversion has involved constantly working 
against the academic misuse of the term spiritual to 
refer to whatever gives one meaning in life. That 
definition, rooted in Christian existential theology (for 
example, the work of Paul Tillich), is a misappropria- 
tion of the natural language word, reflecting the 
philosophical and theological inclinations of many 
academics. But it is a false and confusing characteri- 
zation of the concept in common English. You 
should also note that spiritual in this traditional, non- 
material sense is at the heart of the word supernatural. 
The words are not identical in meaning, but believing 
in one entails believing in the other. 

Anyway, in 1974 I had wondered whether SP 
with a presence was the only such anomalous experi- 
ence giving rise to supernatural folk belief — belief in 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



spirits being the main such belief. Beginning in 1974 I 
searched for broader implications, lessons that New- 
foundland's "Old Hag" might teach us about other 
supernatural traditions. Could other supernatural 
beliefs also arise from experience rather than vice- 
versa? In 1974, the year I returned from Newfound- 
land, Raymond Moody published Life After Life (1 st 
edition, Atlanta: Mockingbird Books), "Actual case 
histories that reveal that there is life after death." 
Moody coined the term "near-death experience" and 
described the NDE as common among resuscitands. 
The immediate skeptical response, especially from the 
medical community, was that this could not be com- 
mon or "we would have known about long ago!" My 
SP work showed me the flaw in this reasoning, and a 
little fieldwork quickly showed me that the NDE 
seemed to be another case of experientially based 
supernatural belief. Subsequent research reporting 
NDEs from other cultures and other times showed 
that it fit the Experiential Source Hypothesis in the 
same way that SP with a presence does. At about the 
same time I found the work of W. Dewi Rees, M.D., 
a Welsh physician whose study published in The Brit- 
ish Medical Journal (1971) showed that visits from the 
spirit of a deceased loved one are common among 
the bereaved. Contrary to contemporary psychiatric 
thinking, which had labeled such experiences symp- 
toms of pathological grieving, Rees showed that these 
visits (now called "after death contacts," ADCs) were 
consistently associated with less indications of depres- 
sion and better resolution of grief! Continued re- 
search over the past 30 years has confirmed Rees' 
early conclusions, and the characterization of the 
experiences in the psychiatric literature has changed 
dramatically. 

During my 30 plus years at the College of Medi- 
cine I made the study of modern resistance to the 
facts of what I came to call "extraordinary spiritual 
experiences" (ESE's, as opposed to ordinary experi- 
ences interpreted spiritually), as much a part of my 
research as the experiences themselves. I found the 
cultural context within which the experiences occur, 
dominated not by science per se, but by materialistic 
philosophical beliefs assumed to be inextricable from 
science, to be essential to the study of the experi- 
ences. Among my conclusions has been the convic- 
tion that science and well-established scientific 
knowledge do not contradict "folk beliefs," either 
those about spirits or folk medical beliefs such as 
those that underlie herbalism in the treatment of dis- 
ease. I realized that what was at issue was the cultural 
authority of science, that that authority had been 

24 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



excessively extended over the past century or so. This 
did not amount to a disagreement with either the 
scientific method or the well-established findings of 
science. In fact, I came to believe that what was 
needed to begin to appreciate the remarkable knowl- 
edge of folk traditions was better science, more rigor- 
ous and less biased. 

John Morehead: What are the various interpreta- 
tions that are brought to the phenomenon in the cul- 
tures in which it is found? 

David Hufford: That's a really interesting question. 
There is variety, but a constrained variety. The inter- 
pretations center, as you might imagine, on the in- 
truder. In almost all cases this entity is described as 
evil or at least threatening. It may be interpreted as a 
sorcerer or a ghost or demon or some other kind of 
supernatural, such as a vampire. In many locations it 
is assumed that more than one kind of creature can 
do this, such as both sorcerers and ghosts. The defini- 
tive characteristics of these categories, of course, are 
not unambiguously presented in the SP experience. If 
the intruder is recognized as a particular living per- 
son (which seems rare) then it is understandable that 
it will be interpreted as a sorcerer. If the attack is sex- 
ual, which seems infrequent but it does happen, and 
if there is a term such as incubus or succubus, that 
will be applied. If the attack occurs in a house be- 
lieved to be haunted, which is common, then the in- 
truder is generally assumed to be a ghost. When fea- 
tures of an attack do not obviously suggest one kind 
of entity or another, then local categories fill in, such 
as the aswang (Tagalog) in the Philippines. This re- 
markable consistency and similarity across cultures is 
a product, obviously, of the robust and consistent 
cross-cultural pattern of the phenomenology of SP. 

John Morehead: Let's focus specifically on how the 
phenomenon is interpreted in Western cultures where 
secularism, advances in the neurosciences, and skep- 
ticism toward religious or spiritual experiences, are 
prevalent. How have paranormal or other spiritual 
interpretations been received in this context? 

David Hufford: The conventional view in anthro- 
pology, folklore and other disciplines has always been 
that all experience is somewhat ambiguous, so the 
values and assumptions resident in one's culture will 
determine one's interpretation of events. This is the 
central understanding of the Cultural Source Hy- 
pothesis (CSH), and it extends even beyond interpre- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



tation to perception in many theories (e.g., the 
Whorf-Sapir Hypothesis). As you note, the conven- 
tional view in the modern academic world is philo- 
sophical materialism, especially with regard to mat- 
ters of spiritual belief and religion, which are as- 
sumed to be very ambiguous. But ironically, the Cul- 
tural Source Hypothesis accounts for the academic 
interpretation of SP, not for the interpretations found 
among most that have experienced SP! Despite evi- 
dence to the contrary most academics assume that 
somehow prior learning, presumably through cultural 
processes, yields expectations that produce the con- 
tent of all sorts of spiritual experiences. This is what 
has been called the universal hermeneutic approach; 
it is illustrated by the influential work of philosopher 
Steven Katz. Katz, who was most concerned with 
"mystical experiences," insisted that visionaries only 
experience what they have been taught to experience. 

Contrary to modern intellectual assumptions, 
most subjects in the modern Western world, the disen- 
chanted world to use Weber's term, interpret SP events 
as spiritual or "paranormal." This is because the 
events are, in fact, minimally ambiguous. And the 
available interpretations for an intruder who can walk 
through walls and paralyze its victim (etcetera) are 
very few: hallucination or something spiritual or 
"paranormal." The SP consciousness is very lucid, 
unlike dream consciousness, and many of the obser- 
vations (e.g. the physical environment) made in this 
consciousness are veridical. This clear sense of reality 
warrants this interpretation for most subjects. Of 
course, there is also the fact that we now know that 
the "disenchantment" of modern consciousness has 
been greatly over-rated! 

John Morehead: In the conclusion of your Transcul- 
tural Psychiatry essay you state, "that there is nothing 
specific within our scientific knowledge of [sleep pa- 
ralysis] that contradicts spirit interpretations." Given 
our growing understanding of the brain through the 
neurosciences, can you expand a bit on what you 
mean and how there may be connections here be- 
tween scientific knowledge of the brain in religious 
experience and a spiritual interpretation of that ex- 
perience? 

David Hufford: Another good question! In consid- 
ering the relationship between scientific knowledge 
and spiritual belief we need to be scrupulous about 
the meaning of the term contradiction. Two proposi- 
tions are contradictory only if they negate each other, 
that is, if it is the case that if Proposition 1 is true 

25 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Proposition 2 must be false, not just that Proposition 1 
challenges Proposition 2 or suggests that Proposition 2 
may be wrong. The scientific proposition "that the 
Earth is billions of years old" negates the Young 
Earth Creationist proposition "that the Earth is 6,000 
years old." If one of these propositions is true, the 
other must be false. Logical analysis requires that we 
understand the meaning of the terms involved. 
Therefore, the hermeneutical idea that "6,000 years" 
in scriptural terms might mean something very dif- 
ferent from what we mean by it today removes the 
contradiction but makes the proposition rather mean- 
ingless. 

A proposition that would negate the traditional 
interpretation of SP would be "that there are no im- 
material spirits." If that were true, it would negate 
the traditional idea "that the shadow intruder in SP is 
a spirit of some kind." These propositions would con- 
tradict each other. But "that there are no spirits" is 
not a scientific proposition. There are no scientific 
experiments, nor can we easily imagine one, that 
would establish this proposition. If it were true "that 
the intruder in SP is a spirit" that would not contra- 
dict any scientifically established knowledge. It would 
not be relevant to the mechanistic REM explanation 
of the cholinergic "switch" for SP atonia. On the 
other hand, the knowledge that the SP phenomenol- 
ogy is independent of cultural context does contra- 
dict the conventional social science use of the Cul- 
tural Source Hypothesis (CSH) to explain SP. But this 
use of the CSH has no valid empirical base, being 
more a reflection of ideology than a scientifically de- 
rived conclusion. 

Scientific method and scientific knowledge about 
sleep are very useful in understanding SP, but they do 
not include some crucial information that is widely 
available in folk tradition, and that can be checked 
empirically. In this sense the two traditions are com- 
plementary. But brain science at present no more ex- 
plains the consistent phenomenology of SP than folk 
tradition explains its neurophysiology. 

Common spirit experiences do not show that the 
Earth is flat, that germs do not cause disease, etc. 
They do not contradict and are not contradicted by 
modern knowledge. The observation that many peo- 
ple with modern knowledge reject these beliefs does 
not constitute a contradiction. Much more common 
than contradiction is the idea that modern knowledge 
makes supernatural belief unnecessary by providing 
superior explanations for the same observations. This 
is the argument from parsimony, or Occam's Razor. 
This claim has its roots in the old notion of super- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



natural belief as consisting of primitive explanations 
for observations of natural phenomena. 

The kind of direct perceptual "spirit experi- 
ences" reported in SP (and NDEs, ADCs, et cetera) do 
not inherently offer an account of any natural phe- 
nomena. If they did there would be the possibility of 
contradicting scientific knowledge. What they do of- 
fer is an account of some of the characteristics of 
spirits and their relationship to humans. All conven- 
tional theories of such experiences treat them as hal- 
lucinations or illusions and rely on assumptions of 
cultural sources to account for their patterning, be- 
cause no psychological theories exist that explain (or 
even acknowledge the existence of) complex halluci- 
nations having a broad, cross-cultural, perceptual 
stability. However, these experiences cannot be ac- 
counted for by cultural models because of their cross- 
cultural distribution. Therefore, even on grounds of 
parsimony, modern knowledge does not conflict at all 
with the most basic beliefs that follow from such ex- 
periences. 

John Morehead: In your research you have noted 
similarities between the sleep paralysis phenomenon 
and out-of-body and UFO abduction experiences. 
Are there any similarities or parallels to other things, 
and what does this tell you about sleep paralysis? 

David Hufford: One partial exception to the 
spiritual/ "paranormal" interpretation, arising from 
modern ideas, is the notion that these events are 
"screen memories" for alien abduction. Contrary to 
what some researchers have claimed, this remains a 
minority interpretation, and it relies on the spurious 
idea that these "screen memories" conceal a forgot- 
ten scenario that can be retrieved through hypnotic 
regression. The prevalence and distribution of SP 
with a presence, historically and cross-culturally is 
entirely at odds with this idea. The same is true for 
the tragic error of treating SP as a screen memory for 
repressed memories of sexual abuse, or as the root 
cause of Sudden Unexplained Nocturnal Death Syn- 
drome (SUNDS) among Southeast Asian men. 

The similarities in these cases come largely from 
the outside observer rather than the subject. In both 
alien abduction and sexual abuse scenarios the pres- 
ence of a threatening intruder in the bedroom is 
similar. The pressure of someone lying on you may 
be similar to sexual abuse, and the feeling of leaving 
your body, present in a substantial minority of SP 
events, resonates with the alien abduction scenario. In 
SUNDS the impression of impending death common 

26 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



in SP is a similarity. But these are tenuous similarities. 
In SUNDS, for example, the subject actually dies, but 
all epidemiological and medical evidence indicates 
that people simply do not die from SP. Also, SP 
OBEs do not involve trips to alien space ships, unless 
the SP experiencer is subject to extensive interroga- 
tion under hypnosis by a UFO researcher. And only a 
small — but important — fraction of SP cases involve 
sexual aspects. These and other misattributions of SP 
result from widespread ignorance of SP, and they can 
be VERY destructive. I have dealt with them at some 
length in my Transcultural Psychiatry article. 

What we learn from the erroneous connections of 
SP with a variety of unrelated phenomena is that 
even robust, consistentiy stable classes of spiritual 
experience will be the subject of extreme efforts at 
assimilation to interpretations that seem more "mod- 
ern" than the common understanding of subjects. 
Even alien abduction, as unconventional as it is, pro- 
vides a modern sounding account in contrast to 
ghosts! These reinterpretations of SP are not so dif- 
ferent from the interpretation of near-death experi- 
ences as delirium or after death contacts as hallucina- 
tions of pathological grieving. In all cases the fit of 
the data to the interpretation is poor, but the goal 
seems to be modernization rather than objective ac- 
curacy. 

John Morehead: In your Transcultural Psychology es- 
say you discuss "the persistence of spirit beliefs in 
modern society despite the cultural and social forces 
arrayed against them." You argue that this may be 
accounted for due to "transcendent, spiritual experi- 
ences." How do you see sleep paralysis functioning as 
a "core spirit experience?" 

David Hufford: By core spiritual experiences I mean 
perceptual experiences that (a) refer intuitively to spirits 
without inference or retrospective interpretation, (b) 
form distinct classes with stable perceptual patterns, 
(c) occur independently of a subject's prior beliefs, 
knowledge or intention (psychological set), and (d) are 
normal (i.e., not products of obvious psychopathol- 

ogy)- 

Here perceptual experiences means episodes of 
awareness that subjectively appear to be observations 
rather than inferences or emotional states. Most SP 
experiences (about 80% in my survey data) include a 
"spirit (that is, an apparentiy non-physical) intruder," 
and many develop into complex scenarios of assault. 

It should be obvious, then, why I consider this a 
spiritual experience: it usually involves a spirit (the 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



intruder), and when SP produces an OBE it presents 
the experience of being a spirit. Despite the typically 
ambiguous meanings of spirituality so common among 
intellectuals today, lexical research has overwhelm- 
ingly shown that in English for many centuries spiritu- 
ality refers to spirits. By core spiritual experience, I 
mean that such experiences provide a central (core) 
empirical foundation from which some supernatural 
beliefs develop by inference. You may recall that at 
the beginning of my career I set out to ask whether 
traditional supernatural beliefs might have some ra- 
tional and empirical elements. The discovery of core 
spiritual experiences answers that question with a 
clear yes. 

John Morehead: Are there any new trajectories in 
your research in this phenomenon? What can we look 
forward to in your future work in this area? 

David Hufford: Remarkably it seems my original 
trajectory remains both viable and productive. I still 
want to assess and understand the empirical and ra- 
tional grounds of widespread spiritual beliefs. I want 
to find additional core spiritual experiences. For ex- 
ample, in 1985 I collaborated with Genevieve Foster 
in the writing of her memoir of a particular kind of 
mystical experience (The World Was Flooded with Light, 
University of Pittsburgh Press). There is reason to 
believe her experience is a member of another core 
experience set, but we have very little relevant data. I 
would love to pursue that. I am trying to understand 
the common intellectual resistance to traditional 
spiritual belief both from the materialist side and 
from the theological side. Keep in mind, even though 
core spiritual experiences are found in most religious 
traditions around the world, they are either absent or 
severely constrained within modern, mainstream re- 
ligion. I also want to understand fully the role of 
medicine, especially psychiatry, in stigmatizing and 
suppressing this topic in the modern world through 
psychopathological theories. 

Out of each of those strands, my central desire is 
to facilitate a change in the modern understanding of 
spirituality, a change that needs to reform both sci- 
ence (including medicine) and religion. A change that 
recognizes that Weber's disenchantment of the world 
did not, in fact happen, and for good reason. The 
world we live in is far more interesting than we have 
been taught. The spiritual aspect of the world de- 
mands the attention of educated and sophisticated 
thinkers, not the kind of anti-empirical dogmatic de- 
nial of human spirituality that we see today. The pub- 

27 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



lie needs to know that if they have a near-death expe- 
rience or a visit from a deceased loved one that they 
have good reason to feel the consolation that comes 
naturally with such experiences, and not the anxiety 
imposed by modern sanctions against spiritual expe- 
rience. They need to know that if they have a scary 
experience of SP it does not mean they are crazy OR 
that they can't tell the difference between waking and 
sleeping Other cultures throughout the world have 
knowledge that helps to deal with SP. We should not 
be the only ones left in ignorance. The ignorant and 
irrational rejection of spirituality so common among 
intellectuals in modern society makes the public vul- 
nerable to all sorts of cult claims and religious ex- 
tremism. I would like to contribute to changing these 
things. I am far from alone in this, and I see the 
change coming. I hope to live long enough to con- 
tribute to reaching the turning point! 

Biographies 

John W. Morehead has an MA in 

intercultural studies from Salt Lake 
Theological Seminary. He applies 
his academic background in religion 
and cultural studies to his work in 
popular culture. In this area he has 
taught courses in theology and film, 
and contributed to various works 
including Halos & Avatars, Butcher 
Knives & Body Counts, Horror Films 
of the 1990s, an essay on Matrixism 
for The Brill Handbook of Hyper- 
Real Religion, and served as as co- 
editor and contributor to The Undead and Theology. He sits 
on the editorial board of GOLEM: The Journal of Religion 
and Monsters. In addition to his pop culture interests, he 
also conducts research, writes, and lectures on new relig- 
ions, world religions, and interreligious dialogue. John also 
edits TheoFantastique (www.theofantastique.com). 

David Hufford is Professor and 
Director at the Doctors Kienle 
Center for Humanistic Medicine at 
the Penn State College of Medi- 
cine (Hershey), where he has ap- 
pointments in Medical Humanities, 
Behavioral Science, and Family 
and Community Medicine. He is 
Adjunct Professor in the Program 
of Religious Studies at Penn and 
is currently providing leadership in 
an initiative to establish a center 
on Spirituality and Health in Penn's 
School of Medicine. His primary 
research interests, which incorpo- 
rate perspectives on applied folklore and theory, are in the 
areas of alternative health systems and folk belief and 
practice. His book, The Terror That Comes In The Night, 
explores the experiential basis for belief in the supernatu- 
ral. David teaches courses on the Ethnography of Belief, 

Vol. 4 No. 3 





Folk and Unorthodox Health Systems, and Human Diver- 
sity in Healthcare. 



Publications: 




and 



dicologu 




EDITED B? 

KIM PAFFENROTH 
JOHN W. MOREHEAD 



https://wipfandstock.com/store/The Undead and 
Theology/ 





Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

The Experiencing Brain 

Charles D. Laughlin 



Sociocultural anthropologists typically ignore the 
brain. Whole books on anthropological theory are 
still written, many dealing with psychological issues, 
but which make no mention of the neurosciences, or 
neuroanthropology for that matter (e.g., Moberg 
2013). This is a curious form of neglect considering 
that everything anthropologists talk about with re- 
spect to culture, enculturation and acculturation per- 
tains to activities of neurophysiological systems. As a 
consequence of this neglect, anthropology fails to 
utilize the rich body of research that could inform 
them about their scope of inquiry. Among other 
things, any act of consciousness cannot be any more 
complex, any more intelligent, any more creative or 
insightful than the neurophysiology mediating the 
act. We cannot perceive anything that our senses 
cannot detect. We cannot understand more than our 
brain can model. We cannot experience anything that 
our brain cannot structure and comprehend. We 
cannot process information that our brain is not de- 
signed and prepared to process. The preparedness to 
experience is fundamentally 'wired-in.' Indeed, every 
moment of our stream of experience is being medi- 
ated by the cells in our brain that originate as inher- 
ited neural structures (neurognosis, or neurognostic 
structures; see Laughlin, McManus and d'Aquili 
1990) which become altered and conditioned 'so- 
cially' in such a way that the experience or physical 
act can be produced and understood in local cultural 
terms. 

The Brain World 

In short, we experience between our ears. Our world 
of experience is constituted by and occurs entirely 
within our brain. Hence, our world of experience 
might as well be called our brain world. The extra- 
mental world - the world as it exists apart from our 
experience or knowledge of it - we may call the real 
world. 

Our brain world consists of neural models of the 
real world that mediate experiences we project out 
upon the real world by way of our feed-forward cog- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



nitions and actions. Interaction with the real world 
results in a feedback loop which our brain uses to 
correct its models. Models are made up of neural 
circuits by the tens of thousands that organize them- 
selves in such a way that they mediate a percept, an 
image, a thought, a feeling to the 'mind's eye.' The 
brain is both the producer and audience of the mind- 
movie that is our ongoing stream of experience - the 
producer and audience of our brain world. 

Why didn't I simply call the brain world the 'in- 
ternal world' and the real world the 'external world?' 
The reason is because our brain and our body (apart 
from our modeling of them), are part of the real 
world. We are both beings in the real world and 
minds that experience and model both our inner 
selves, and happenings in the external world. We are, 
empirically speaking, a special object in the real world 
in that we may experience ourselves both from the 
outside in (I see my fingers moving over this key- 
board) and from the inside out (I feel the pressure 
inside my fingers as they press against the keys). Only 
conscious beings can do that. Moreover I can only do 
it for myself. I do not have access to you from the in- 
side out. The closest I can get to this is the experience 
of empathy. 

When we think about things, reach conclusions, 
make judgments, have insights, feel things — the expe- 
riences and their mediating neural structures exist 
only within the confines of our bodies. The repercus- 
sions of these experiences occur in the real world, but 
are limited in their effects to that part of reality that is 
our self - our being. If I fantasize having a gourmet 
meal with Sharon Stone, the effects of this internal 
process remain internal to my body. But if I act upon 
it - say, I pick up the phone and make reservations 
for me and Sharon at Le Bee Fin, and then whip off 
an invitation by email to Sharon at 
www.hollywoodcelebrities.com, then the effects of my 
brain world activity transcend my body and have im- 
plications in external reality. Perhaps a while later 
several beefy men in white coats show up to escort 
me to a nice, quiet sanitarium. This was not my in- 
tended outcome, obviously. I had imagined that 

29 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Sharon would leap at the chance to have a super 
meal with someone who's intelligence, and humble- 
ness for that matter, are equal to her own. Alas, the 
real world is such a tragic bummer! 

The Real World 

That's the hell of it! The real world is transcendental 
relative to my mind and will. That is, there is always 
in all things more to reality than I can know or con- 
trol. While I am focused on this rather than that, real- 
ity is all happening all the time. While I am busy 
knowing this, there is a whole lot of that going on 
simultaneously. Meanwhile, reality has power over my 
body and my mind. Reality is forever resisting my will 
and conditioning my acts. That's called being 'realis- 
tic' Being realistic means I realize that the real world 
is characterized by its obduracy relative to my inten- 
tions, and I act accordingly. If I try to walk through a 
wall without the benefit of a door, I will come up 
against the obdurate nature of reality. While I may 
imagine or dream that Sharon and I are having a 
jolly time discussing string theory over our terrine de 
saumon aux epinards, attempts to do so in reality 
may well prove disappointing, even disastrous for me. 
Also, if I try to solve a problem - like, try to recall all 
the movies Sharon has starred in —and I can't seem to 
do it, it is my brain itself that is the obdurate reality 
that is thwarting my will. Folks my age encounter that 
problem all the time. I am demanding more of my 
brain than it can accomplish at the moment. Assum- 
ing I am relatively sane, the feedback from reality will 
at least lead me to alter my expectations, and perhaps 
adjust my discernment between fantasy and reality. If 
I am not able to make those adjustments, then the 
fellows in the white coats may conclude, with good 
reason, that I am 'crazy' 'out-of-it,' 'wacko,' so forth. 

Neurocognitive adaptation has to do with our 
encounters with the obdurate nature of the real 
world - both physical reality and social reality (solid 
walls other people and social conventions). Indeed, 
much of early development in the baby has to do 
with exploring the somatic and sensory limits of ob- 
duracy - the obduracy of the baby's own body and 
its local environment. 

Reality also impresses itself on our brain world 
through feedback about what is really possible. I like 
to use the term affordancy for this feedback, a term 
coined by the famous psychologist, James J. Gibson, 
to conceptualize the active interaction between expe- 
rience and reality. Affordancy is what reality provides 
for our adaptation, whether the effects be 'good or ill' 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



- reality provides we critters both aliment and poison. 
The development of knowledge about the real world 
is the process by which the brain builds models from 
our stock of inherited neurognosis that match - that 
anticipate and accurately depict - what is afforded by 
the world. Over there I see an object that looks like a 
'chair.' The range of objects that we recognize (liter- 
ally re-cognize) as being chairs is vast, and are pre- 
cisely those objects we interpret as 'sit- able.' Some 
objects are also 'stand-on-able.' Some 'chairs' are also 
'stools' that are cognized as both 'sit-able' and 'stand- 
on-able.' Many 'chairs' do not afford 'stand-on- 
ability' and are thus not also 'stools,' and we would be 
silly to use them as stools. Learning all about that is a 
'chair' and what is not is part of our development. So 
too is which women are 'date-able' and which are 
not. Alas, Sharon is, for me at least, not only 'un- 
date-able' but probably 'un-meet-able.' As the Bud- 
dha taught, life is dukkha, 'suffering,' 'struggle,' or as 
I prefer to translate it, 'a bitch.' 

What is obdurate and affordant is not a quality of 
reality so much as it arises during the interaction be- 
tween an animal and its environment. In other words, 
obduracy and affordancy depend upon the nature of 
the animal, as well as the nature of the environment 
of the animal. A stick lying over a stream may afford 
adequate support ('bridge-ability') for a colony of 
ants wishing to cross over, but not for a dog. Flowers 
afford information in the ultraviolet range for honey 
bees, but not for nearly hairless apes who cannot per- 
ceive in that range of the spectrum. A river may ob- 
durately thwart our crossing, but not a beaver's or an 
elephant's. That rock may afford me a weapon, but 
not for my dog Toby who has no hands. A small body 
of water may be a puddle to an elephant that walks 
right through it, a pond for us nearly hairless apes 
who have to walk around it, and an ocean to an 
earthworm who may well drown in it. 

Another way to see affordancy and obduracy is as 
the consequence of causation. As Arthur Peacocke 
(2010:254) has written, '...to be real is to have causal 
power' — the locus of control over what causes what 
in the interaction between a brain world and reality is 
external to the will of the animal. Our brain world is 
the result of our cognizing our real self and our real 
environment. Our world of experience is mediated 
by neural networks that are themselves part of real 
entities — real bodies — that are in turn embedded in a 
real world of systemic, causal efficacy (to use White- 
head's term). We know extramental reality because 
we run up against the causal efficacy of both our lo- 
cal environment and our own bodies. If we take our 

30 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



next breath and all there is to breathe is water, then 
we will drown. The locus of control over causal effi- 
cacy is external to our brain world. 

Brain World/Real World 

The brain world/real world dynamic is a setup for 
several systematic epistemological and ontological 
errors frequently encountered among peoples across 
cultures. 

/. Mangling the Brain World and Real World 

First of all, obduracy and affordancy are really ob- 
verse qualities of reality in interaction with the devel- 
oping brain world. Both our real body and the exter- 
nal world present, not only as sensory experiences (I 
see my hands, I hear my voice), but also as obdurate 
(I can't fly in air no matter how hard I flap my arms, 
but I can fly in water) and affordant (I can pick up 
and handle all sorts of objects — i.e., they are 'grasp- 
able' and 'manipulable') limits to our intentionality, 
and thus operate to guide the development of our 
knowledge about our physical being, our world and 
the interactions between the two. We encounter these 
qualities daily, as do all animals. We only become 
aware of them per se when we run up against either 
resistance to our intentions or new opportunities we 
had not recognized before. Once we have adapted to 
(adjusted our neural models of) obdurate and affor- 
dant features in the world, we generally 'adapt-out' 
and lose awareness of the distinction between our 
experience and extramental reality - the distinction 
between experience and real world fuzzes out and we 
assume our experience to be reality. We all remember 
when we learned to tie shoelaces and neckties, and 
how the actions became automatic once we had 
learned them. In a sense, we construct ourselves dur- 
ing the course of development and adaptation into a 
kind of automaton, a 'wet' robot who's will is to some 
extent autonomous from our consciousness. 

The point here is that people everywhere quite 
naturally mangle the distinction between brain world 
and real world - regardless of cultural background. 
We normally operate as though the world of our ex- 
perience — the movie in our head — is reality, when it 
is never more than an adaptational rendition of real- 
ity. Our world of experience is, and can only be, real- 
ity as depicted by our brain world for the consump- 
tion of our brain world. Our world of experience can 
only be our particular point of view. After all, I am 
looking at this bright monitor while typing and quite 
naturally - and falsely - assume that the light is 'out 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



there,' when it is in fact 'in here,' inside my brain 
world. Light and color is how my brain world inter- 
prets and presents to itself electromagnetic energies 
of a particular range of the visible spectrum to my 
mind's eye. A congenitally blind person cannot nor- 
mally experience light and color. His or her brain 
world is devoid of light, just as the normal human 
brain world is devoid of ultraviolet images that are 
part of the honeybee's perception, or the electromag- 
netic images apparent in the electric eel's perception. 

2. The Brain-World and the Transcendental 
Nature of Reality 

Second of all, because we normally and quite natu- 
rally project our brain world onto reality, we thereby 
lose track of the fact that the real world is always 
transcendental relative to our models, comprehen- 
sion, perception and intentions. With respect to self- 
awareness and self-understanding, we experience 
ourselves as we think we are, as we imagine we are, as 
we feel we are. We always know our self from a point 
of view, and that point of view is always partial. I can 
see the front of this monitor, but not the back. In fact 
I cannot see all the sides of anything at the same 
time. The great painter, Pablo Picasso played with 
this natural limitation to perception in many of his 
cubist works, like seeing a woman's face from both 
the front and side at the same time. By the same to- 
ken, I can never experience my entire being. Most of 
the real me is hidden to my perception or introspec- 
tion. My being is forever a transcendental mystery 
unto myself. 

Naturally, if we were to change our point of view 
on ourselves, our model of ourselves would likely 
change. For instance, if we make a study of our body 
scientifically, we soon discover we are less a 'person' 
than we are an ecosystem, a foraging ground for our 
microbiome (Wilson 2004; Marples 1965). Few of us 
take into account the fact that trillions of microor- 
ganisms live on us and inside us, and make our real 
body their home. Just which organisms live where on 
us depends on many factors that affect locations on 
and in our bodies as niches. Temperature variation, 
moisture, pH, chemicals present and absent, available 
forage, access to light, how often and with which 
products we wash, and so forth. Different places on 
the skin have different populations of different mi- 
crobes. So too in our gut. It is estimated that some- 
thing like 100 trillion microbes live on us and in us. 
There are roughly 10 times the number of microbes 
on and in us than we have cells in our body! In one 
study of 26 adult humans, it was found that an aver- 

31 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



age of 46,000 living organisms dwell under each fin- 
gernail. [Ha! Think about that next time you scratch 
an itch!] Yet we never think about our self as an eco- 
system. Again, our own body is a transcendental ob- 
ject to our brain world. Even our brain is a transcen- 
dental object to our brain world. We could spend the 
rest of our lives studying the human body - including 
our own body and its brain - and never come to the 
end of knowledge about our being. 

It does not matter what aspect of reality toward 
which we turn our attention, there is more to it than 
we can ever know - and it is all happening all the 
time. We can study baseball, ceramics, nematode 
worms, black holes, ocean tides, legumes, robotics - it 
really doesn't matter, for we will never come to the 
end of it unless our brain world stops the process of 
inquiry. Stopping the process of inquiry is precisely 
what the brain world is designed to do. We naturally 
will turn toward, and become interested in novelty 
until at some point our urge to understand the nov- 
elty wears thin, and then we 'close' our model and 
carry on. It does not matter that there is an endless 
amount of information yet to be learned, our brain is 
designed to stop inquiry when it has adapted to the 
novelty and rendered it redundant and sufficiently 
meaningful. The more intelligent the animal, the 
longer and more energetic will be our scrutiny of 
novelty. Chimps will study a novel object longer on 
average than will a baboon or other monkey. Hu- 
mans will study novelty longer than will a chimp. But 
inevitably we lose interest and our model of the pre- 
viously novel object or happening closes. We have 
adapted to it. We have modeled its obdurate and af- 
fordant nature. 

There is an interesting Buddhist meditation that 
teaches one a lot about this process. In some circles it 
is called 'doing a Patthana' (named for the last book 
of the Abhidhamma Pitaka). The Patthana is a 
lengthy discourse on causation, and isolates through 
contemplative methods some 24 types of causality 
(paccaya) that are involved in any and all experiences. 
Doing a Patthana involves meditating upon any phe- 
nomenon - the simpler the better, like an apple 
standing on a table top - and parsing out all the 
causal relations necessary for that experience to be 
occurring before the mind at that moment. Like any 
meditation of substance - and this one gets really 
complex, really quick! - one has to actually do it to 
really understand the point of it. Suffice to say, no 
matter what phenomenon you meditate upon, you 
end up with the entire universe, as well as its history 
and to some extent its future. In other words, you 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



never come to the end of the causation. This is one of 
the powerful meditations that can lead eventually to 
the realization of totality - the realization that every- 
thing is causally interconnected, and that nothing 
whatever is separate or independent of the All. This 
is a very rare level of systemic comprehension. Very 
few people anywhere are able to comprehend their 
world in such terms. 

3. Invisible Causation in the Real World 

To repeat: the real world and everything in it is tran- 
scendental relative to our ability to model it within 
our brain world. Another reason that this is the case 
is that most of the causality operating in the real 
world is hidden from us -invisible to our senses. This 
is especially true of causal relations between other- 
wise visible things and events. If a causal relationship 
is very proximal both in space and time, then we can 
be accurate in our understanding of many of its ele- 
ments. The other car ran a red light and T-boned us. 
I throw a stone and a few moments later see the 
splash in the lake. But most causation in the real 
world is relatively distant from our point of observa- 
tion. We adapt to gravity, but we can neither see grav- 
ity nor can we totally comprehend gravity. What we 
actually do is fill in the gaps with concepts and theo- 
ries. I don't mean just scientific ideas and theories 
here. I mean stories and explanations developed in 
each and every culture on the planet to account for 
the invisible aspects of the world. This is the stuff 
myths are made of. 

For instance, the Navajo people of the American 
southwest hold that all perceivable things in the world 
have normally invisible, causative, spiritual aspects 
that are imagined as 'Holy People' — for example, the 
Mountain People, the Star People, the River People, 
the Rain People, the Corn People, and so on. For so- 
phisticated Navajo thinkers, these Holy People are 
anthropomorphized symbols for the usually hidden 
and vital element within all things, and which tradi- 
tional Navajo philosophy equates with 'Wind' (nilch'i; 
see McNeley 1981). People themselves also have such 
a hidden dimension called 'the Wind within one' (nil- 
ch'i hwii'siziinii). All these Winds are really part of 
the one all-pervasive, all-encompassing Holy Wind. 
Winds are never distinct entities and there is energy 
flowing in and out of even the most enduring and 
solid objects. It is the coming and going of wind that 
accounts for the tapestry of reciprocal causation typi- 
cal of their understanding of the cosmos. The choice 
of 'wind' as the central metaphor is an explicit rec- 
ognition - common to many cultures on the planet - 

32 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



that there are forces that normally cannot be ob- 
served, save by inference from their effects. You can- 
not see the wind, but it can blow your house down in 
a storm. 

It is very much the function of myth in more tra- 
ditional societies like Navajo to reveal and explicate 
the invisible dimensions of the world. The hidden 
energies that are the essence of the world are given a 
face — a countenance that may be contemplated, that 
is 'pleasing to the mind,' that may be enacted in ritual 
(like mystery plays and healing ceremonies) and that 
may be imagined in daily life as the efficient cause of 
significant phenomena and events (see Davis-Floyd 
and Laughlin 2013). For those members who are well 
versed in their society's mythological system, the core 
myths and their various symbolic extrusions are all- 
of-a-piece. They form a single, ramified 'cognitive 
map' within the context of which events — even 
events in the modern world of global politics and 
economic affairs — make sense and are easily related 
to both other events in the contemporary world, and 
archetypal events that unfold in that timeless era of 
mythological mysteries. 

4. Finite Brain-World, Infinite Real-World 

The relationship between our brain world and the 
real world is thus one of a model to the real thing 
being modeled. Only, in this case, the model is very 
finite, much localized and very simplified, and reality 
is transcendental and infinite. As I have said, our 
brain world, by way of its nature of sometimes being 
conscious, tends to be focused on this rather than 
that, while reality is happening all the time - and in- 
cidentally reality never sleeps. Moreover, most of 
what is happening in reality is invisible to our brain 
world. Those trillions of microbes just keep foraging 
about our body-ecosystem, doing their individual and 
collective thing -which, by the way, keeps our body- 
ecosystem healthy most of the time - and we are 
blissfully unaware of it. Those vast hoards of mi- 
crobes might as well not be there, for all the attention 
we pay them. Yet their existence and their activities 
are real and they have real effects in the real world. 
Some NASA scientists have wondered whether we 
humans can actually live permanentiy in space colo- 
nies because of our dependence upon microorgan- 
isms that we can only see under powerful microscopes 
(check out Pyle et al. n.d.). 

The real world isn't localized. Locality is defined 
by conscious beings mentally adapting to their envi- 
ronment. Reality on the other hand has no center, no 
focus, no locality. Modern physicists will tell you that 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



the entire universe is implicated in every event, no 
matter how small or large. We come to know and 
model our world from our being outwards. We are 
the center of our own self-constructed universe. 
When a newborn baby focuses on objects in her envi- 
ronment, they are objects that are very close. New- 
borns cannot see clearly beyond a few feet. Only 
gradually do their senses extend outwards in their 
quest for sensory patterns to identify and store in 
memory. When a new worker honeybee begins to 
forage outside the hive, she does so in gradually in- 
creasing circles outward from the hive, going no fur- 
ther afield than she can cognitively map and find her 
way back home. Meanwhile, the real world is all 
there all the time, a plenum void with infinite dimen- 
sions and literally mind-boggling complexity. 

Conclusion 

One of the implications of this neuroanthropological 
view of experience is that 'relativist,' or 'constructiv- 
ist' theories of culture simply won't wash anymore. 
The brain world is never a blank slate. It is exquisitely 
structured from fetal life onward. Cultures are varia- 
tions on a theme. Most of the essential elements of 
experience are the same for every normal human on 
the planet. Interpretations will vary locally, as will 
emphasis upon this or that state of consciousness. 
Some cultures like ours will typically ignore their 
dream life, while other cultures consider dreaming 
essential to their way of life (Laughlin 201 1). Yet eve- 
ryone on the planet dreams every night, and the 
structural properties of dreaming are universal. An- 
thropologists continue to ignore the neurosciences at 
their peril, for as the burgeoning neuroscience disci- 
plines emerge and master their scopes of inquiry, an- 
thropological theory will be left further and further 
behind. 

References 

Davis-Floyd, R.E. & Laughlin, CD. (2013). The Power 
of Ritual. New York: Random House/Schocken. 

Laughlin, CD (2011). Communing with the Gods: Con- 
sciousness, Culture and the Dreaming Brain. Brisbane: 
Daily Grail. 

Laughlin, CD., McManus, J., & d'Aquili, E.G. 
(1990). Brain, Symbol and Experience: Toward a JVeu- 
rophenomenology of Human Consciousness. New York: 
Columbia University Press. 

33 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

Marples, MJ. (1965). The Ecology of the Human Skin. 
Springfield, IL: Thomas. 

McNeley J.K. (1981). Holy Wind in Navajo Philosophy. 
Tucson, AR: University of Arizona Press. 

Moberg, M. (2013). Engaging Anthropological Theory: A 
Social and Political History. London: Roudedge. 

Pyle, Barry et al. (n.d). 'Microbial Drug Resistance 
and Virulence (MDRV). 'http://www.nasa.gov/ 
mission_pages/ station/ research/ experiments/ 
MDRV.html#description. 

Wilson, M. (2004). Microbial Inhabitants of Humans. 
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 



News & Recent Publications of Interest 

"Painful and Extreme Rituals Enhance Social Cohesion and Charity" 
http://www.psypost.org/2013/06/painful-and-extreme-rituals-enhance-social-cohesion-and-ch 

"Vatican to Announce John Paul II 'Miracle'" 
http://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/worldnews/europe/vaticancityandholysee/10129593/Vatican-to-announce- 

John-Paul-ll-miracle.html 

"Hearbeat Used to Generate Out-of-Body Experience" 
http://www.newscientist.eom/article/dn23694-heartbeat-used-to-generate-outofbody-experience.html#.UcLv 

v2BZ8eN 

"Shaman 'Rainmaking' Center Discovered in South Africa" 
http://news.yahoo.com/shaman-rainmaking-center-discovered-south-africa-114745962.html 

"Dreams Cloud Brings lASD's Annual Psi Dreaming Contest Online" 
http://www.prweb.com/releases/201 3/6/prweb1 0792903.htm 

"Is Spirituality the Result of a Combination of Hallucinations and Happiness?" 
http://wikkorg.wordpress.com/2013/06/12/is-spirituality-the-result-of-a-combination-of-hallucinations-and-ha 

ppiness/ 

'"Neurons to Nirvana' Makes the Case for Deeper Scientific Research into Psychedelics" 
http://motherboard.vice.com/blog/neurons-to-nirvana-filmmakers-talk-about-scientific-research-into-psyched 

elics 

"Prehistoric Rock Art Maps Cosmological Belief" 
http://phys.org/news/2013-06-professor-prehistoric-art-cosmological-belief.html 

"The Greeks who Worship the Ancient Gods" 
htt p ://www. b be . co . u k/n ews/m ag az i ne -229726 1 0 



Biography 

An anthropologist by trade, 
education and inclination, Char- 
les Laughlin taught the subject 
at Carleton University, Ottawa, 
Canada, for 25+ years. He re- 
tired in 2001. Among other 
things, being an anthropologist 
allowed Charlie to live with dif- 
ferent peoples all over the 
planet, including African pastor- 
alists in East Africa, Tibetan 
lamas in Nepal and India, and 
Navajo Indians in the American 
southwest. He naturally learned 

lots of things, including how cultures influence the states of 
mind of people, and how culture is both an adaptational 
strength and a trap for individual minds seeking the truth of 
being and existence. 




Vol. 4 No. 3 



34 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



UFOs and Other Anomalous Phenomena 
Connections, Beliefs and Perspectives 

Jose Banuelos 




Anomalous and often perplexing phenomena that have not been explained in a conclusive manner 
have been a part of human experience for a long period of time. Through this time such phenom- 
ena have been interpreted in different frameworks (supernatural, paranormal, religious, magical, 
etc.), this has contributed to the formation of various beliefs and fields of study that have individu- 
ally dealt with and advanced theories and "refined" interpretations. Historically a few researchers 
have endeavored into the unknown with an open mind and have found parallels between some 
manifestations and events, although the contribution from those researchers has been a valuable 
one, it must also be acknowledged that it has been limited, a comprehensive or holistic perspective 
to the anomalous phenomena and its relationship to humanity and the environment has yet to ap- 
pear. The way we [humans] tend to form and hold on to our beliefs appears to be deterring our 
progress towards a solution. The purpose of this paper is to encourage an open-minded, inclusive, 
and multidisciplinary approach to anomalous phenomena. 



Ideas and Beliefs 

Humanity harbours the most various opinions 
and fantasies about Truth, morality and 
Beauty, even about prosperity and (most curi- 
ous) even about health (Koneczny 1962: 153). 

Before an adequate and reliable approach to the 
anomalous and paranormal can be developed, it is 
important to consider a fundamental, influential and 
often overlooked aspect: belief. The manifestations of 
the phenomena (anomalous aerial objects, strange 
creatures or beings, visions, abductions, etc.) can be 
traced back to at least the beginning of recorded his- 
tory. It appears that in early times the phenomena 
were not really considered to be unexplained, on the 
contrary, they received numerous interpretations that 
found a place in the beliefs of the times, or gave way 
to new ones (although mainly of a religious or spiri- 
tual nature). 

Until fairly recently some interesting events have 
taken place: 1) there has been an increase in openness 
and tolerance towards alternative ideas, 2) beliefs that 
were once confined to certain geographical areas 
have found their way unto others, 3) a more objective 
and historical approach to these subjects has been 

Vol.4 No. 3 



taken by a few researchers, and 4) technology has 
facilitated the communication of recent phenomena. 
This series of events have encompassed religion, 
spiritualism, occultism, witchcraft, paranormal phe- 
nomena, etc. resulting in a surge of alternative ideas 
and beliefs that are more or less revivals and amal- 
gamations of "ancient knowledge" tailored according 
to the intellectual, technological and social conditions 
of the present, but ones that do not necessarily repre- 
sent reality in a more profound or complete way. 
However, closer or farther from the mystery as they 
may be, these new beliefs are bound to influence and 
change our future conditions, just as ancient religions 
and beliefs greatly shaped history through a subtle 
influence upon human thought and (consequently) 
human action. Our first observation is that the inter- 
pretations that have been attributed to anomalous 
phenomena have (directly or indirectly) influenced 
humanity at least since recorded history began, and 
the way we interpret anomalous phenomena today 
can have an effect on our future. 

Humanity has reached an intellectual and tech- 
nological level that allows us to communicate over 
vast distances in real-time, gather great amounts of 
information from worldwide sources, analyze the data 
and make inferences based on what was gathered. In 

35 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



view of these circumstances it would seem that 
greater discussion, proposal and objective research 
looking for conclusive data on anomalies would take 
place, but this has seldom occurred, it seems as if 
open-mindedness is still scarce. Meanwhile, research 
of related topics is becoming more strenuous, tedious 
and sometimes even confusing due to the overwhelm- 
ing amount of information that is becoming avail- 
able. This information has not facilitated the explana- 
tion of the phenomena, but according to Horgan: 

The perennial philosophy, postmodernism, 
negative theology, transpersonal psychology, 
neurotheology, gnosticism, and neo-shamanism 
all insist in their own ways that there is an irre- 
ducible mystery at the heart of things. So does 
science (2003, p.218). 

Although no discipline has unveiled this mystery yet, 
they all have evoked one recurring element: believers 
and skeptics, but where are the unbiased and open- 
minded who dispassionately and carefully consider 
the alternatives? Even with the "pseudo-sciences" 
where one would think researchers would keep an 
open-mind, many have comfortably settled for a view 
that has remained unaffected by other equally or 
even more plausible alternative explanations, the ex- 
traterrestrial origin as an explanation for UFOs is an 
example, in his investigation on this subject Keel no- 
ticed that: 

Man's tendency to create a deep and inflexible 
belief on the basis of little or no evidence has 
been exploited. These beliefs have created tun- 
nel vision and blinded many to the real nature 
of the phenomenon (1970, p. 7). 



Approaching the Unknown 

In certain ways the study of the anomalous or para- 
normal has followed a similar approach as science; it 
has been divided into "sub-disciplines" that are dif- 
ferentiated by the type of manifestation and/ or their 
given interpretation. Variability has mostly been 
taken to represent distinct and unrelated phenomena. 
Each discipline has been further fragmented by dif- 
ferent theories that are backed by advocates who 
dedicate substantial time and effort to advance a the- 
ory. The content that has been derived from such 
theories and research has (for the most part) been less 
than scientific, in the sense that it has not been based 
on rigorous and unbiased investigation and/or re- 
porting. This has mostly led to speculation, contro- 
versy and to further obscure that which was already a 
mystery. 

With the anomalous it seems that a reductive ap- 
proach does not seem very favorable as it has not yet 
been determined how much the phenomena encom- 
passes and/ or how far it permeates, yet, the reductive 
approach is the road most researchers have taken. 
Few have considered the idea and investigated the 
phenomena with the possibility that the various kinds 
of manifestations could represent parts of a whole, a 
single source or coordination between sources. Some 
researchers are not even interested in the various 
types of phenomena (Mizrach (2013) noted this 
about many ufologists), Cannon wrote about this di- 
vision within ufology: 

Many investigators study only sightings and 
physical traces such as landings, and stop there. 
Other investigators study only abductions and 
stop there (1999, p.8). 



Even though Keel observed this in the 70s it can be 
argued that it still applies in the present. There are 
probably just a few ufologists who would consider an 
alternative explanation to the phenomenon, Randies 
suggested that they should end their search for 'be- 
ings that, in the end, appear to be illusory' (2013, 
p.3 1). But even if the various anomalous phenomena 
or their interpretations were all but illusory, the ef- 
fects on humanity are certainly not. 'I have come to 
see that the abduction phenomenon has important 
philosophical, spiritual, and social implications' 
(Mack 1995, p.3), the same can be said about experi- 
ences of a religious, mystical, psychedelic and para- 
normal nature. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



Others (although open to some paranormal phenom- 
ena) would appear have a limit to what they will be- 
lieve to be possible, of the 1966 Mothman sightings 
of West Virginia, Bishop comments: 

There was so much weirdness connected with 
this story... that many UFO investigators and 
historians refuse to take the case seriously. That 
is a shame, as there may be keys here to un- 
locking the interconnected nature of the UFO 
phenomena with other fortean issues (n.d.). 

Considering that humanitycould be dealing with a 
'level of thought that is superhuman', Aime Michel 

36 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



pointed out that 'neither the absurd nor the contra- 
dictory must ever be excluded as such' (Bowen 
ed.l969,p. 255) 

Researchers that have looked at variability as 
separate parts of one "system" have gone on to un- 
cover some interesting parallels. It is important to 
remember that open-mindedness and knowledge in 
other fields have played a significant role in discover- 
ing these similarities. In the paranormal field, Keel 
was one of those researchers, he wrote: 

History, psychiatry, religion, and the occult 
have proven to be far more important to an 
understanding of the whole than the many 
books which simply recount the endless sight- 
ings of aerial anomalies (1970, p.6). 

At some point it starts to become apparent that (as 
Michel so aptly stated): 'the rule is to think of every- 
thing and to believe nothing' (Bowen 1969, p.253). 

Making Connections 

We have already seen that one of the most frequent 
consequences of anomalous phenomena has been the 
creation of beliefs. But belief has not always come 
from the interpretation of the events; it has also come 
from supposed communication. In ancient cultures it 
is said that shamans and priests received messages 
from their deities, people that reported to have had 
direct contact with angels or messengers of god 
abound in religions and spiritual beliefs, numerous 
are the cases of automatic writing and mediums that 
have received information, and the UFO contactees/ 
abductees who have received messages from "inter- 
stellar beings." These are all examples of supposed 
communication with some sort of supernatural being, 
intelligence or mind. Many cults, spiritual movements 
and religions have been established by direct instruc- 
tion or inspiration from this contact. Sociologists 
Glock and Stark stated: 

All religious experiences, from the dimmest to 
the most frenzied, constitute occasions defined 
by those experiencing them as an encounter 
between themselves and some supernatural 
consciousness (quoted from Vallee 2008, p. 14). 

It is worth noting that it does not appear to be the 
case that the purported contactees of "otherworldly 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



beings," "ascended masters" and "messengers of 
god" have doubted the information they have re- 
ceived. 

Physical, psychological and physiological similarities 
in the experiences of the anomalous can be found by 
researching the many cases through history. For ex- 
ample, parallels have been identified in the kidnap- 
ping stories of fairy tales, "alien" abductions, witches 
meetings with the devil, and various other 'mythical' 
stories, even the religious stories of angels taking 
people to mountains or heaven (Bejarano 2013; Keel 
1970). Vallee wondered why the supposed aliens be- 
haved 'like the denizens of fairy tales and the elves of 
ancients folklore?'(2008, p.6). Mack also noticed the 
similarities, he stated: 

The UFO abduction experience, while unique 
in many respects, bears resemblance to other 
dramatic, transformative experiences under- 
gone by shamans, mystics, and ordinary citi- 
zens who have had encounters with the para- 
normal (1995, p.441). 

Time and its influence on space have always been of 
importance to humanity and it appears to play an 
important role in the experiences of anomalous phe- 
nomena as well. In various cases of UFO encounters, 
alien abductions, fairy kidnappings, time does not 
seem to elapse in the ordinary manner in which we 
are accustomed to, it would seem more analogous to 
dreams and some psychedelic experiences. 

Sound and light are also important elements of 
the anomalous. There is the whistling, swishing, 
humming, hissing, or eerie throbbing sound that is 
usually described in close encounters with UFOs and 
abduction cases. In religious /spiritual literature and 
art we find a parallel in the angels and messengers of 
god who were depicted as luminous beings, some- 
times with a sounding trumpet, as well as the chariots 
or clouds from which some of them came. In the 
fairy stories there is the singing that was used to en- 
chant humans, fairy circles have become crop circles. 
Strange sounds and lights are also part of the polter- 
geist phenomena. 

The study of close encounters with 'aliens' and 
abduction cases has shown that many people who 
had this experiences often reported other parapsy- 
chological, psychic or poltergeist phenomena: hear- 
ing voices speaking from within oneself, unexplain- 
able sounds, rapping, lights flickering, locked doors 

37 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



opening by themselves etc. (Fowler 1982; Mack 1995; 
Keel 1970). 

Cryptozoology and ufology are usually consid- 
ered as separate and unrelated, yet, there have been 
many encounters with strange creatures and beings 
(not associated with the usual description of 'aliens') 
in relation to UFOs which could suggest a link be- 
tween both types of phenomena (Bowen 1969, Keel 
1970). Curiously, Mack (1995) noticed that to many 
abductees, 'aliens' first appeared as animals, and that 
'the connection with animal spirits is very powerful 
for many abductees' (1995:18). 

During an investigation into SPH (spontaneous 
human combustion), Randies and Hugh were 'led, 
somewhat unexpectedly, into contact with UFOs' 
(2013, p. 31), they associated the physical/ 
physiological effects of the UFO phenomena with 
energy (especially static charge), effects that in the 70s 
Keel attributed to electromagnetism. The malfunc- 
tion of electronic and mechanical equipment has 
been connected with various anomalous phenomena. 

It is also interesting to note that many of the pat- 
terns and consequences on humanity that can poten- 
tially be attributed to anomalous phenomena have 
been discerned through the meticulous examination 
of worldwide historical events, it seems as if the phe- 
nomena becomes more apparent through its long 
term effects (social consequences) only long after they 
have influenced human action. This in itself should 
be a very important aspect for research as we may be 
dealing with a subtle but potent influence on human- 
ity (no matter what the source may be). For this rea- 
son Vallee repeatedly urged scientific research into 
the UFO phenomena. 

At this point there is one aspect that could link 
shamans with enlightened people, abductees, fairy 
stories, psychedelic experiences and paranormal phe- 
nomena: a profound change in perception. Through 
history some people have looked for it, others have 
suddenly been exposed to it, and most have had their 
attention swayed towards it (whether they have no- 
ticed it or not) by curiosity or some kind of associa- 
tion. 

Hide and seek 

Through history the different phenomena has appar- 
ently been manifesting, leaving just enough amount 
of evidence (be it visions, signs, etc.) so as to evoke 
certain interpretations that led to various beliefs, 
manifested with enough variability and confusion so 



as to avoid arousing investigation towards itself, but 
inducing an impact so great that myths and beliefs 
based on the events have endured for centuries. In his 
investigation on mysticism Horgan mentions: 

Even the most fantastical ghost stories, includ- 
ing the old stories of religion, can serve a pur- 
pose... [They] can remind us of the unfa- 
thomable mystery at the heart of things (2003, 
p.235). 

Be it by accident or design, the manifestations of and 
the beliefs that the anomalous phenomena evoke do 
not shed light on the mysterious source and its inten- 
tions (if any). It tends to avoid objective explanation 
in a way that one could seriously conclude (but not 
reliably prove) that there is intelligence behind it; we 
are left with "coincidences" and "absurdities." 

History has taught us that when an idea turns 
into an inflexible belief and open-mindedness ceases, 
human action can be controlled (for better or worse) 
by authority, paradoxically, it could also be true that a 
vast amount of information based on so many alter- 
native ideas can make objective research very difficult 
or next to impossible (even with the technology avail- 
able today), therefore, one important consideration in 
the study of the anomalous phenomena is that of 
consciously keeping check on our own beliefs, their 
influence on our view of reality, and our expectations 
when doing research, because to some degree our 
published or communicated views and results 
(whether they are correct or not) can contribute to 
the ideas and beliefs of others, and we as 
researchers/authors could be somehow influenced 
and used for this purpose. 'There are times when co- 
incidental circumstances make me feel like a pawn in 
some complex but predetermined chessgame' wrote 
Fowler (1982, p. 131). When investigating the Moth- 
man sightings, Keel realized he could be manipulated 
by the phenomena, he termed this aspect of it the 
"reflective" effect (1975). We can also hint at a more 
subtle yet interesting occurrence with Mack: 

I will devote more attention in this book to the 
transformational and spiritual growth aspects 
of the abduction phenomenon... There are 
several reasons for this decision... most inter- 
esting, I think, is my personal experience as a 
psychiatrist dealing with abductees: I seem to 
receive more information of this kind in my 
work with abductees than, apparently, do other 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



38 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



investigators. It is not altogether clear why this 
is so (1995, p.31). 

In 1988, long before writing her book about the ab- 
duction phenomena, Cannon also had a 'strange oc- 
currence': 

During the night I had the distinct and unfa- 
miliar feeling that an entire block of informa- 
tion had somehow been inserted into my 
head... I knew the concept dealt with the ex- 
planation that should be included in my book 
on UFO cases, which had not yet even started 
(1999, pp.12-13). 

It took ten years for Cannon to accumulate enough 
information to form a book, yet she affirmed that: 'it 
definitely followed the concept given to me in 1988' 
(1999, pp.14). 

In light of these incidences it might also be im- 
portant for authors and researchers of these topics to 
consider the events in their own lives and look for 
subtle but certain influences that have led them to an 
interest in these topics, as well as the circumstances 
that have led to the information and ideas they seek 
to convey to others. We might find some interesting 
parallels and "coincidences" in our personal history. 

Conclusion 

Regrettably anomalous phenomena has been for the 
most part researched and analyzed partially and with 
much bias, yet, in the process many strong ideas have 
been formed and currently prevail. The average per- 
son tends to associate UFOs with extraterrestrial life, 
poltergeists with ghosts, ghosts with deceased people, 
cryptozoology with undiscovered or ancient crea- 
tures, fairies and elves with myth, etc. 

It should be clear by now that the study of the 
anomalous requires (among other things) impartial 
researchers who are dispassionate towards the result 
as long as the truth is revealed, who are unbiased to- 
wards alternative explanations, capable of admitting 
errors and adjusting research efforts accordingly, who 
are familiar with different anomalous phenomena 
including historical events and are knowledgeable in 
as many "scientific" fields as possible. 

The anomalous phenomena have proven that 
humanity is most vulnerable in one area, in its need 
for belief. Humanity can prosper or decay based on 
its beliefs and anomalous phenomena has been 



shown to be at the core of various (if not all) of them. 
It is time we look at all manifestations and events with 
an open and unbiased mind, trying to uncover what 
has been behind our very own ideas and motives, and 
for what purpose. 

References 

Bejarano, F. (2013). 'Supernatural Abductions: UFO 
and Folklore Narratives. Paranthropology: Journal of 
Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal' [On- 
line], 2(3), 8-13. Available from: 
http:/ / www.paranthropologyjournal.weebly.com 
/free-pdf.html [Accessed 22 May 2013]. 

Bishop, G. (n.d. ). 60 years of UFOs: The top ten cases. 
ForteanTimes [Online]. Available from: 
http:/ / www.forteantimes.com/ strangedays/ ufofi 
les/ 519/ greg_bishops_ufo_top_ten.html [Ac- 
cessed 12 June 2013]. 

Bowen, C. ed. (1969). The Humanoids. New York: 
Henry Regnery. 

Cannon, Dolores (1999). The Custodians. Arkansas: 
Ozark Mountain. 

Fowler, Raymond E. (1982). The Andreasson affair, phase 
two. New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. 

Horgan, John (2003). Rational Mysticism: dispatches from 
the border between science and spirituality. New York: 
Houghton Mifflin Company. 

Keel, John A. (1970). Why UFOs: Operation Trojan 
Horse. New York: Manor Books. 

Keel, John A. (1975). The Mothman prophecies. New 
York: Tor Books 

Koneczny, Feliks (1962). On the plurality of civilizations. 
London: Polonica Publications. 

Mack, John E. (1995). Abduction: Human encounters with 
aliens. Revised edn. New York: Ballantine Books. 

Mizrach, Steven (2013). 'The Para-Anthropology of 
UFO Abductions: The case for the UTH.' Par- 
anthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to 
the Paranormal [Online], 4(2), 4-18. Available 
from: 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



39 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



http:/ / www.paranthropologyjournal.weebly.com 
/free-pdf.html [Accessed 22 May 2013] 

Randies, Jenny (2013). Alien endgame? ForteanTimes 
(UFO Casebook), February, pp. 31. 

Randies, Jenny (2013). Fire from the sky. ForteanTimes 
(UFO Casebook), June, pp. 31. 

Vallee, Jacques (2008). Messengers of deception: UFO 
Contacts and Cults. 2008 Edition. Brisbane: Daily 
Grail Publishing. 



Biography 

Jose Banuelos has been interested in ancient cultures, 
science, art, and anomalous phenomena since he was 
ten years old. In his early 20s he searched for a cen- 
tral truth in various esoteric disciplines and teachings, 
later his interest in archeology brought him in contact 
with various alternative theories regarding the source 
of ancient culture and myth. His recent look into the 
UFO and abduction phenomena has culminated in a 
reflection upon all his previous explorations. He has 
learned that an unprejudiced and inquisitive mind is 
fundamental in approaching mysteries. 



New Publication of Interest: 
Breaking Convention: 
Essays on Psychedelic Consciousness 

A multidimensional trip into psychedelic consciousness, science and culture. Topics covered 
range from Neolithic worldviews, prehistoric rituals and Amerindian epistemology to weapon- 
ized hallucinogens, religious freedoms, trip lit and the 



death of the '60s dream. This collection of 22 original 
essays transects a wide range of disciplines to offer 
empirical, mystical, imaginal, hermeneutic, queer, 
phenomenological and parapsychological perspec- 
tives on the exploration of psychedelics, taking in 
scientific debates on MDMA, manifestos, policy chal- 
lenges, anaesthetic revelations and communications 
from the herbs along the way. 

Featuring contributions from: 

Cameron Adams, Joseph Bicknell, Ras Binghi 
Congo-Nyah, Nese Devenot, Rob Dickins, Rick 
Doblin, Jon Cole, Val Curran, Kevin Feeney, Amanda 
Feilding, Tom Froese, Jonathan Hobbs, Mike Jay, 
Axel Klein, Reka Komaromi, Beatriz Caiuby Labate, 
Andy Letcher, Luis Eduardo Luna, David Luke, 
Kirkland Murray, Peter Oehen, Andy Parrott, Vit 
Pokorny, Ffion Reynolds, Andy Roberts, William 
Rowlandson, Ben Sessa, Angela Voss, and Anna 
Waldstein 




Artwork by Blue Firth 
Edited by Cameron Adams, Anna Waldstein, David Luke, Ben Sessa & Dave King 

Published by Strange Attractor Press 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



The Culture of War, "Afterlife Conscious Minds," & 
Morphogenetic Fields: The Past Soundscapes of an 
American Civil War Battlefield 

John C. Sabol 




Introduction 

In this paper, I will present a particular society, the 
"culture of war" of the American Civil War period, 
where, in a particular situational setting (a battlefield), 
the sense of hearing (and directed listening) domi- 
nated the external sensory experience of men, and 
directed their actions in specific spaces. This sensing, 
defined as the "bodily" means of gathering informa- 
tion, was an acoustemology of experience, a particu- 
lar way of knowing the external environment as one 
experienced "seeing the elephant" on an American 
Civil War battlefield."? According to Clinton (2009), 
"the deathbed of a loved one was perhaps the most 
hallowed of Nineteenth Century ritual settings" 
(2009:4). The "Good Death" was a prepared death, 
surrounded by family at home, and a burial in the 
family plot. The American Civil War battlefield 
changed that. The ritual was never completed, in 
many instances, for the soldiers who fought and died 
on these American Civil War battlefields. 

In an important ethnographic study in the an- 
thropology of the senses, Kathryn L. Geurts (2003) 
investigated the cultural meaning system and senso- 
rium of the Anlo-Ewe-speaking people of Southeast- 
ern Ghana. In her book, she introduces a new aspect 
of embodiment as a paradigm for anthropological 
fieldwork. In Anlo culture, there is little relevance for 
the five-senses model that pervades Western Euro- 
Anglo-American cultural traditions. Geurts's work 
documents the Anlo culture's use of sensory experi- 
ence, and involves a theory of inner states, and their 
particular way of defining external experience. On 
an American Civil War battlefield, where I have con- 
ducted ethno-archaeological "ghost excavations" for 
a number of years, this theory of "inner states," as a 
way of defining "external experience," comes into 
focus for my research on "apparitional experience" as 
it is perceived today on the Civil War battlefield. The 
lack of the "good death" was a contributing factor, I 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



propose, to this contemporary "apparitional experi- 
ence." This paper concerns one such investigation, 
the engagement at Burnside Bridge on the Antietam 
Battlefield near Sharpsburg, Maryland. 

Burnside Bridge: An American Civil War 
Battlefield Engagement 

The battle of Antietam was fought on September 17, 
1862. It was the single bloodiest day of combat in 
American history with more than 26,000 casualties. 
The engagement at Burnside Bridge, fought on the 
southeastern part of the battlefield, was a horrific 
engagement which lasted for five hours on the morn- 
ing and early afternoon of the 17 th . At Burnside 
Bridge, more than 1 1 ,000 Union troops assaulted the 
bridge five times before occupying the Confederate 
positions on the opposite bank of Antietam creek. 
That bridge was defended by less than 300 Confed- 
erate soldiers. Because of the heroics of the Confed- 
erate defenders, the engagement at Burnside Bridge 
has been called the "Thermopylae of the Civil War" 
(Tucker 2000:154): 

"At Antietam a relative handful of ragged and 
barefoot 2 nd and 20 th Georgia soldiers per- 
formed one of the most important military 
feats of the war by defending Rohrbach's 
Bridge (later called "Burnside Bridge") for 
most of 17 September 1862. These Georgians 
were truly Spartans in gray, who fought against 
impossible odds (Tucker 2000:154). 

Because of this highly emotional defense, the large 
amount of Union casualties (600), in a narrow and 
confining space, and the time period involved, it was 
thought that the landscape in and around Burnside 
Bridge was a good site to explore the possibilities of 
recording the "remains" of any sonic elements of a 
Civil War soundscape that may have been recorded 

41 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



onto the environment. The landscape has received 
little change since the Civil War. It is protected by 
employees of the National Park Service, and access 
into and out of the area is controlled by park rangers. 
Special permission was needed to conduct an investi- 
gation there at night. 

The stone bridge itself afforded a possible re- 
cording device, and Antietam creek was another pos- 
sible source. There have also been numerous reports 
of people experiencing "anomalous" manifestations 
in the area ("voices," "shadow figures," other "visual" 
anomalies), as well as having personal "apparitional 
experiences." During our non-evasive "ghost excava- 
tion," we hoped to record some of these "residual" 
elements. What we did record, however, was far more 
than a "residual" soundscape. We have recorded, in 
context, the possible "voices" of specific soldiers who 
fought and died at Burnside Bridge on September 17, 
1862. 

Social and Mental Fields: Are These Evi- 
dence of an "Afterlife Conscious Mind? 

How we make ourselves human, be human, and re- 
main human even, perhaps, after the physical death 
of the body and brain, was (is), in one particular "cul- 
ture" (the "culture of war" of the American Civil 
War), through an acoustemological means. In this 
context, I agree with Geurts's assertion that "a cul- 
ture's sensory order is one of the first and most basic 
elements of making ourselves human" (2003:3). If 
sensory order is a patterned field that gives relative 
importance to different senses through which a soci- 
ety learns to perceive and experience the world, then 
the Civil War soldier learned a particular modality of 
sensing and interacting in this "culture of war." That 
particular modality was acoustemological, forming a 
particular and learned way of knowing how and 
when to move and act on a battlefield. It is this 
learned pattern of knowing that may survive, I pro- 
pose, after physical death. 

An American Civil War battlefield was primarily 
a soundscape, not a landscape, for the common 
"foot" soldier. The intense (and blinding) firepower 
that was generated onto the environment, in mostly 
restricted spaces, obscured the vision of the land- 
scape setting. This battlefield soundscape was linked 
to particular "external experiences" (hearing specific 
"soundmarks" in particular spaces/temporalities). 
This "audio-vision" (Chion 1994) prompted a specific 
"inner state" which Jordania (201 1) has termed "bat- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



tie trance." This "battle trance," I propose, created 
specific cultural and mental "fields" (Sheldrake 2012) 
of the "culture of war" of the American Civil War, 
imprinting these "fields" onto the physical environ- 
ment. 

As part of the "culture of war," these soldiers 
developed their sonic abilities as a means to know the 
"external experience" of combat on a Civil War bat- 
tlefield. This knowledge not only served them well in 
combat, it created a sociocultural "tradition" that 
involved a sensibility and sensitivity to particular con- 
textual sounds or "soundmarks" that were recognized 
by the soldier in combat situations. This "cultural 
sense" has been described by anthropologist Robert 
Desjarlais as "a lasting mood or disposition patterned 
within the workings of a body" (1992: 150). 

Does this "lasting mood" that became patterned 
in Civil War combat also last after the death of the 
physical body? Does it become a fundamental social 
and mental field of an "afterlife conscious mind" that 
survives today on a Civil War battlefield? Does it 
form an historical field pattern of individual (and 
collective) social habits that remain as vestiges and 
traces of the "culture of war"? If so, do these fields 
become expectations (and manifestations) of what it 
is to remain human in a given time and place from a 
particular time and place? 

I propose that the "auditory streams" that we 
have recorded during extensive fieldwork at Burnside 
Bridge on the Antietam battlefield in Maryland 
(USA) might indicate the survival of some form of 
social/mental field as patterned acoustemological 
presences of this "culture of war." If this acoustemo- 
logical modality, as a sensorial battlefield "external 
experience," did become encoded as a field pattern, 
then it should manifest as a unique auditory reper- 
toire and configuration of the soundscape in particu- 
lar battlefield spaces. This additional acoustical ele- 
ment should, to use Steven Feld's terminology, "lift up 
over" the "soundings" of contemporary sonic ele- 
ments and vocalizations of the soundscape, and be 
contextual to the "soundmarks" and auditory streams 
that would have occurred on the battlefield (in par- 
ticular spaces) in 1862. 

These "fields" (as "acts"), if they continue today, 
were first developed by "habitual" drilling off the 
battlefield. They were re-established in the battlefield 
soundscape by auditory cues which repeated the 
sounds and behaviors of "habitual" drilling 
(prompted by bugle calls/drum rolls/commands 
(such as "roll-call"), etc.). These "fields," as memory 
"tracks," surfaced in battle and produced Inherent 

42 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Military Probability (I.M.P.) behaviors, or what the 
soldier would have done in particular situations on 
the battlefield. It is these cultural (I.M.P. behaviors) 
and mental ("battle trance") fields of the "culture of 
war" (as "inner states" and "external experiences" in 
combat) that survive, I propose, as "forms of life" of 
an "afterlife consciousness" on these battlefields. 

These "cultural and mental fields," learned from 
habits ("drilling"), and reinforced on the battlefield 
(through sensory cues/"soundmarks"), begot a rela- 
tionship between the acquisition of cultural knowl- 
edge (I.M.P. behaviors of the "culture of war") and a 
focus on a particular human sensory modality (audi- 
tory). This acoustemological sense of knowing how to 
act became a natural (albeit habitual and mundane) 
process, even in battle, and it became an essential 
part in creating a specific human "community" (a 
Civil War "company" of soldiers) who identified 
themselves as a "band of brothers" (both literally and 
figuratively). 

The acquisition of these "social and mental 
fields" (I.M.P. behaviors/"battle trance") involved a 
process, I propose, of "self-resonance" (cf. Sheldrake 
2012) in combat, repeating past behaviors learned in 
drills and cued to particular "soundmarks." Geurts 
(2003:238) states: 

"Self-processes, including those of sensory at- 
tention and orientation, require effort or 

agency and intentionality The sensorium 

helps assure that notions of the person both 
differ culturally, yet appear natural to those 
who hold them." 

This "self" process creates, I propose, a tangible link 
between shared cultural practices (I.M.P. behaviors of 
the "culture of war"), through physical training of 
bodily experience and auditory flow (drills/ 
soundmarks), and our contemporary performances of 
these traditions (cultural resonance) in a "ghost exca- 
vation" that utilize the repetition of acts of past be- 
haviors (I.M.P.) in particular battlefield spaces (or 
K.O.C.O.A.). 

According to Geurts (2003), there is a "cultural 
installation" (2003:85) inside the sensing body that 
reaches far beyond the individual (or the cultural 
group itself). Do the "cultural and mental fields" of 
I.M.P. behaviors of the "culture of war" reach be- 
yond the physical death of individual soldiers, the 
mid- 19 th c. "culture of war" itself, and what (who) 
remain on the battlefield as both residual and interac- 
tive presences? Does a Civil War "mentality" (as a 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



state of mind) survive? I propose that it does, and 
involves a learning process of "self-resonance" as de- 
veloped by Rupert Sheldrake (cf. 2012). 

Recognition and Recall of Consciousness: 
Past to Future 

Our recent fieldwork at Burnside Bridge on the Anti- 
etam battlefield has recorded a series of audio 
streams that may be indicative of these sonic ele- 
ments of an "afterlife conscious mind" of the "cul- 
ture of war" of mid- 19 th c. America. These audio 
streams were recorded in specific battlefield spaces 
(K.O.C.O.A.). Doing research, as Rupert Sheldrake 
states, "we should make as few assumptions as possi- 
ble" (2012: 12). During our extensive fieldwork at 
Burnside Bridge, we have reiterated the same investi- 
gative (resonating) practices of I.M.P. behaviors, and 
have recorded the same "voices" responding to these 
contextual scenarios, even though these scenarios 
were performed months apart (and with different in- 
vestigative teams) who had little prior knowledge of 
the historical record of the battle and the men who 
fought there. 

In each subsequent "excavation" at Burnside 
Bridge, we have encountered (and interacted with), I 
propose, what Sheldrake has termed "evolving hab- 
its" (2012:85), as a kind of memory. These habits, 
according to Sheldrake, "grow stronger through repe- 
tition" (Ibid: 97). Does a manifestation become pre- 
sent as a consequence of (and influenced by) what 
happened before? Did past drills influence some fu- 
ture behavior on the battlefield? Does present investi- 
gative acts (that are culturally-resonant to a particular 
space and time) influence and cause the manifestation 
of past Inherent Military Probability (I.M.P.) behav- 
iors of the "culture of war" in the form of contem- 
porary intentional acts of an "afterlife conscious 
mind" of a Civil War soldier? I propose that they do! 

This process of habit formation, what Sheldrake 
calls a "morphogenetic field," includes these social 
and mental fields. They are defined as: 

* Social fields: These " co-ordinate the behav- 
ior of social groups" ; and 

* Mental fields: These "shape the habits of 
mind" (Sheldrake 2012:100). 

Do these social and mental fields survive as sonic 
elements of interactive "traces" of the "culture of 
war" of the American Civil War, and manifest at 

43 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Burnside Bridge today as I.M.P. acoustic behaviors of 
soldiers that died in battle more than 150 years ago? 

If a Civil War consciousness survives the physical 
death of men who fought at Burnside Bridge, and we 
repeat elements of their "culture of war" during 
"ghost excavation," then these fields (social/mental) 
could become increasingly habitual as resonating 
behaviors (present to past) are repeated. The record- 
ing of these "repeating" audio manifestations are not 
"paranormal" events, since the "pattern" (as social/ 
mental fields) was already present here in the past. 
The "field pattern" was not "beyond normal" 
("paranormal") since it was originally created in 
drills, and subsequently enacted on the battlefield. 

A "haunting pattern" (the manifestation of these 
social/mental fields), normally "absent," becomes 
actual, given the reiteration of appropriate circum- 
stances (cultural resonance to past fields of the "cul- 
ture of war" through our performance practices). 
The "patterns" of auditory streams that manifest to- 
day may be the social and mental fields that already 
existed as habits of the "culture of war" in the past 
(during drills and subsequent battles in the American 
Civil War). 

Sheldrake suggests that mental causation, the 
field that shapes the habits of mind, runs from the 
present into the past (2012:121). Do our field acts, in 
specific battlefield K.O.C.O.A. spaces (tied to situa- 
tional I.M.P. behaviors), and cued by specific 
"soundmarks," "trigger" the actualization of the past 
social fields of I.M.P. behaviors at Burnside Bridge? If 
an "afterlife conscious mind" survives the death of 
the physical body/brain, then our audio recordings of 
the Burnside bridge soundscape might reflect this. If 
"minds choose among possible futures" (Sheldrake 
2012:129), then these manifestations (coming imme- 
diately following our contextual acts and documented 
on RT-EVP audio recorders) may be purposeful past 
acts that become present as a response to these con- 
temporary "cues" (i.e. habits of past minds cued to 
present acts). 

According to Sheldrake, "minds are closely con- 
nected to fields that extend beyond brains in 
space. . ..and. . ..in time, linked to the past by morphic 
resonance (2012:229). These "minds" as "afterlife 
consciousness" remain attached to the social fields of 
I.M.P. behaviors of the "culture of war" which may 
remain in contemporary space and time due in part, 
I propose, to the retention of residual elements that 
have been recorded onto the Burnside Bridge land- 
scape from the battle that was fought there on Sep- 



tember 17, 1862, and survive as a Civil War "sound- 
scape" today. 

There is no overt_response to those "soundings" 
that remain in the form of "residuals" by this "after- 
life conscious mind" that we have recorded. Perhaps, 
this is because these residuals are not "live" actors. 
However, our "ghost excavation" performance prac- 
tices (as a form of morphic resonance) appear to 
"unearth" a "live" responding presence from the 
past. Is it because we .are not perceived as "residuals," 
but rather "live" performers in the (contemporary) 
"culture of (the) war"? 

In the sense of "external experience" as "appari- 
tional experience," does "normal" becomejhe mani- 
festation of past_and habitual_acts in the present_due 
to morphic resonance? Does this interact, with both 
residual and interactive presence, as resonance, to 
existing and pre-established and past social fields? If 
so, this "normal," then, is not "paranormal." It is 
what actually happens in the present based on what 
had occurred in the past in particular battlefield 
spaces (K.O.C.O.A.). 

If this hypothesis is correct, can we then predict 
when, where, and how these manifestations will occur 
again in the future? If, as philosopher Henri Bergson 
(1946) states, memories are direct connections across 
time, then these manifestations may be the memories 
of I.M.P. habits, "unearthed" during a "ghost excava- 
tion," since the enacted (contemporary) scenarios 
incorporate resonating acts of I.M.P. habits that 
"awaken" past memories that were habitual acts in 
the past. 

If these memories depend upon morphic reso- 
nance, as Sheldrake suggests, then this "afterlife con- 
scious mind" (as entities with memories of habitual 
I.M.P. acts) is influenced by the morphic resonance 
from their own past (as well). This is the "self- 
resonance" of habitual drills transferred to the bat- 
tlefield, and retained in memory after physical death. 
Thus, the "afterlife conscious mind" is awakened in 
the future _by a similar resonance (our contextual sce- 
narios) that "target," during a "ghost excavation," 
this already_existing past and habitual memory sur- 
viving as social and mental fields of I.M.P. behaviors 
of the "culture of war." 

The manifestation of memory (as remembrance) 
occurs as a two-fold process (Sheldrake 2012: 204- 
06): 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



44 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



* Recognition: This is a "similarity between 
past experience and previous experience" ; 
and 

* Recall: This is an "active reconstruction of 
the past on the basis of remembered mean- 
ings." 

Morphic resonance is seen to connect these two 
processes, and this connection forms the basis for our 
"ghost excavation" methodology, and its application 
at Burnside Bridge: 

* Recognition: This helped the Civil War sol- 
dier in battle. He recognized_the "sound- 
mark" cues learned_from drills, taking appro- 
priate actions on the batdefield (I.M.P.). 
These remain as "vestiges" and "traces" of 
the "culture of war" at Burnside Bridge; 

* Recall: This allowed us to "unearth" this pre- 
established memory of social and mental 
fields in specific battlefield spaces 
(K.O.C.O.A.) by using appropriate behav- 
ioral acts (I.M.P.), cued to particular sound- 
markSi 

These recall acts were recognized by the "afterlife 
conscious mind" of these remaining entities as 
learned past acts. They manifest on the contempo- 
rary batdefield where they were previously recog- 
nized by these soldiers in battie from previous habits 
learned in drills. 

Sheldrake believes that "self-resonance from an 
individual's own past is more specific and.... more 
effective" (2012:211). In locations that contain "ves- 
tiges" ("residuals") of the "culture of war" (such as 
those we recorded along the Rohrbach Farm Road, 
an avenue of approach for combat advances in 
K.O.C.O.A. space), as elements of "self-resonance," 
could also account for the continuing presence of 
interactive_manifestations there in the form of an 
"afterlife conscious mind" that survived physical 
death. 

The recognition of these residual battie sounds 
(perhaps related to the topography: water, and the 
stone bridge) could enable a recall of memory of 
I.M.P. behaviors, in this (and other) K.O.C.O.A. 
spaces, by those entities who survive as an "afterlife 
consciousness." Our contextual scenarios, as the use 
of this recognition_of I.M.P. behaviors, would also 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



resonate and serve as the stimulation for the recall of 
these behaviors in battlefield spaces. 

How can manifestations, such as this "afterlife 
consciousness" and related "residuals" (perceived as 
"ghosts"), exist in contemporary reality 150 years 
after the battie? Sheldrake states that "minds are 
closely connected to fields that extend beyond brains 
in space. . ..and. . ..in time, linked to the past by mor- 
phic resonance" (2012:229). When these soldiers 
died, their "mind" as a self-organizing system of 
I.M.P. behaviors that remain on the battlefield, be- 
came "re-animated" by "self-resonance" from their 
own past (the residual sounds recorded on the land- 
scape) and from the future: the "ghost excavation" 
practices that we enact there. This "presence," as 
sonically recorded, has manifested, time and again, 
during_our performance practices in "ghost excava- 
tions" on this battlefield. 

I propose that a mutual learning process (their 
recognition/ our use of recall) is evolving and trans- 
forming the contemporary soundscape at Burnside 
Bridge. This process includes: 

* A past recognition of habits established in 
drills; 

* A past recall that is reinforced by continuing 
present manifestations of residuals that are 
recognized; 

* A present performance practice that recog- 
nizes the importance of resonating acts in 
specific battlefield spaces (K.O.C.O.A.); and 

* A past recognition of these practices as simi- 
lar to those that occurred at Burnside Bridge 
in battie on September 17, 1862, and in drills 
that recall I.M.P. behaviors of the "culture of 
war." 

Those manifestations that occur in conjunction with 
investigative resonating acts, in particular 
K.O.C.O.A. spaces, is, I propose, a form ofjearning 
Do the increase in the frequency of these manifesta- 
tions, during subsequent "ghost excavations," attest to 
this learning process? Does the use of contextual 
"soundmarks," as "triggers," reinforce this learning 
process? Do our contextual acts, portraying those in 
command of troops in particular K.O.C.O.A. spaces, 
identify us as "instructors" in the principles of I.M.P. 
behaviors in the "culture of war"? Julia Hendon is 
an archaeologist, whose book (2010) examines the 

45 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



connections between social identity and social mem- 
ory using archaeological research, states that there is 
a "close connection between identity and memory, 
once the one (identity) becomes something recreated 
over time" (2010:14). She shows how "memory 
communities" assert connections between the past 
and the present. Has our "identity" been established 
by continuously repeating the contextual acts (and 
associated soundmarks) learned in drills and repeated 
on the battlefield in continuing "ghost excavations 
there? Has our resonating acts linked to a past 
"memory community" of the "culture of war" of the 
American Civil War? 

A morphogenetic field has been established, I 
propose, through this learning and "excavation" 
process, and the "field" spreads through time to other 
presences, becoming a multi-layered "memory com- 
munity" that may remain, and may be expanding, at 
Burnside Bridge. This means that these other mani- 
festations, during subsequent "ghost excavations," 
may be a result of recognition and recall as an ex- 
panding morphogenetic field which further extends 
the contemporary reality of what (and who) remains 
at Burnside Bridge as "this happens here" now from 
what happened there on September 17, 1862. 

This possible learning in the "afterlife" is not, I 
propose,_"paranormal" or "supernatural," but learn- 
ing within the framework of what these "ghosts" al- 
ready know: the "culture of war" of the American 
Civil War. Their manifestations are not confirmation 
bias as an expectation of what should occur when 
resonance is used as an investigative practice. It is a 
pre-disposition, a form of "self-resonance" from a 
pre-established "field" laid down in battle, and in 
prior drilling. Do these aural responses and "ques- 
tions" (such as our recording of "Captain, Is that You 
Captain"?) represent "learning"? I propose that they 
most certainly do! You can hear these audio tracks at 
www.ghostexcavation.com (Antietam audio). 

Conclusion 

Our continuing fieldwork at Burnside Bridge, and 
other sites, is an "audiography" of the "culture of 
war" that remain as vestiges and traces on battlefield 
soundscapes today. These "soundings" are embed- 
ded, as part of a layering of presence, a continued 
being in the world in the form of an expanded reality 
in these landscapes. It is a "normal" ethnographic 
sensorium of an "afterlife consciousness" that is im- 



printed on these battlefield spaces. This has created a 
"hauntscape" of multiple immiscible social and men- 
tal fields that potentially can only be accessed, on a 
consistent basis, through cultural resonance. This as- 
sumption implies "the existence of an essential and 
fundamental relationship between ghosts, mind, and 
consciousness" (Beichler 201 1:30). 

Finally, one may ask why these "ghosts" (as an 
"afterlife consciousness"), if they exist, remain on the 
battlefield where they died on September 17, 1862. 
There are several possibilities. I will only mention 
(briefly) a few here. These include the following: 

* Military Orders; 

* The concept of the "Good Death" 

Standard military orders are significant, but often 
overlooked in a battlefield "apparitional experience," 
especially the following: 

* "to quit my post when properly relieved"; 

* "to be especially watchful at night"; and 

* "to talk to no one except in the line of duty." 

Do our "ghost excavation" performance practices at 
night, and contextual to "identities" affiliated with 
commanding officers, allow for the manifestation of 
an "afterlife consciousness" because soldiers, those 
who remain "attached" to their duties, communicate 
with us "in the line of duty"? Do these soldiers re- 
main because they were not accorded the rites and 
rituals of the "Good Death"? According to Clinton 
(2009), "the deathbed of a loved one was perhaps the 
most hallowed of Nineteenth Century ritual settings" 
(2009:4). The "Good Death" was a prepared death, 
surrounded by family at home, and a burial in the 
family plot. The American Civil War battlefield 
changed that. The ritual was never completed, in 
many instances, for the soldiers who fought and died 
at Burnside Bridge. Are these manifestations the "af- 
terlife conscious minds" of those soldiers who remain 
on duty, are vigilant at night, only communicate to 
individuals they_identify as their comrades and/ or 
officers, and who never experienced the "Good 
Death"? This is the theory that I am currently work- 
ing with, as we continue to investigate the manifesta- 
tions of past presence, as "afterlife conscious mind" 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



46 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



of the "culture of war" at Burnside Bridge and other 
battlefields. 



Bibliography 



Beichler, James E. (2011). Apparitions R'Us: Far More 
Than Just a Ghost of a Chance In Academy of 
Spirituality and Paranormal Studies, Inc. 2011 
Annual Conference Proceedings. Bloomfield, 
Connecticut, pp. 24-38. 

Bergson, Henri (1946). The Creative Mind. New York: 
Philosophical Library. 

Chion, Michel (1994). Audio-Vision: Sound on Screen. 
New York: Columbia University Press. 

Clinton, Catherine (2009). Mrs. Lincoln: A Life. New 
York: HarperLuxe. 



Hendon, Julia A. (2010). Houses in a Landscape: Memory 
and Everyday Life in Mesoamenca. Durham, N.C.: 
Duke University Press. 

Jordania, Joseph (20 1 1). Why Do People Sing? Music in 
Human Evolution. Logos. 

Sheldrake, Rupert (2012). The Science Delusion: Freeing 
the Spirit of Enquiry. London: Hodder & Stough- 
ton, Ltd.. 

Tucker, Philip Thomas (2000). Burnside's Bridge: The 
Climatic Struggle of the 2 nd and 20"' Georgia at Antie- 
tam Creek. Stackpole Books: Mechanicsburg, Pa. 



Desjarlais, Robert (1992). Body and Emotion: The Aes- 
thetics Wellness and Healing in the Nepal Himalayas. 
Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. 

Geurts, Kathryn Linn (2003). Culture and the Senses: 
Bodily Ways of Knowing in an African Community. 
Berkeley: University of California Press. 



Biography 

John Sabol is an archaeologist, 
cultural anthropologist, actor and 
author. As an archaeologist, he 
has documented and recorded the 
manifestations of past sound- 
scapes at haunted ruins. As an 
actor, he has appeared in many 
movies, TV series and educa- 
tional TV programming, includ- 
ing the Sci-Fi classic, Dune (1984), 
and the A&E TV series, Paranormal 
State. He has written 16 books on his fieldwork, 
methodology, and his personal experiences on loca- 
tion filming and his work at haunted ruins around the 
world. 



exploring the 

extraordinary 



Fifth Conference 
20th-22nd September 2013, York, UK 

For more information visit 
http://etenetwork.weebly.com 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



47 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

Processes of Experience 

Donnalee Dox 



In contemporary, vernacular practices from sha- 
manic drumming to contemplative dance, the process 
of integrating mental and physical capacities is often 
the hrst condition for opening to extraordinary expe- 
riences. This chapter approaches experience through 
processes by which people identify and cultivate ex- 
periences as special (Taves 2009). The chapter first 
sets out methodological considerations for studying 
experiences cultivated in contemporary, vernacular 
practices, and then focuses on the process by which a 
conceptual metaphor intertwines with physical action 
in the experience of 'opening the heart center' in 
Anusara yoga. 

Modern yoga presents innumerable ways of pars- 
ing and problematizing the experience of mind-body 
integration. An ethnographic study would reveal the 
range and nuances of individual experiences. A re- 
view of yoga styles would compare the experiential 
goals and processes of different yoga styles — from 
relaxing the body, to deep meditation, to mental con- 
trol over the body. This chapter isolates one specific 
experience to access the processes at work in its culti- 
vation. 

Inside Knowledge 

The subjectivity of experience — the inner life of the 
mind and the body — has long challenged the critical 
distance required of scholarly approaches to observ- 
able phenomena. How can we know the insides of 
other people (Taves 2009:63; Slingerland 2008:27, 
151, 304)? The inaccessibility of inner experience has 
proved formidable even in the humanities, which 
long ago exposed the dark ambiguities of the Roman- 
tic soul to the light of identity politics, and showed 
the workings of discourse in the poetics of mysticism. 

Without representational images, neuroscience 
attempts to explain experience by its physiological 
correlates, the gaze of critical scholarship turns to 
social and cultural forms with which people shape 
experience. Expressive forms become the decipher- 
able effects of experience, the sites available for 
analysis and discussion: descriptions, narratives, art, 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



music, dance, and rituals. Practices are especially 
valuable sites because they encompass expressive 
forms and are the very means by which people culti- 
vate experience. Modern hatha yoga is one such 
practice. Yoga's status as a cultural phenomenon has 
focused critical attention on social, economic, and 
cultural aspects of its popularity. Less attention has 
been given to how the practice of yoga identifies and 
cultivates special experiences. 

Approaching Yoga as a 
Cultural Phenomenon 

The shear number of yoga studios, training pro- 
grams, websites, classes, styles, popular market books, 
academic studies, and films about yoga affirm yoga's 
status as a cultural phenomenon. As a fitness and ex- 
ercise regimen, yoga is taught in health clubs and 
gyms and the purported benefits to psychological 
wellbeing have moved yoga into schools, prisons, and 
businesses. Yoga has been a flashpoint for Christian 
sects in America, which may reject yoga in rhetoric 
reminiscent of Nineteenth Century colonialism, or 
refashion yoga postures to align with Christian 
themes Jain 2010). Appropriations of yoga have mo- 
tivated concerns over its origins in Hinduism (Vitello 
2010), while magazines such as Yoga Journal promote 
yoga as a modern lifestyle. Yoga crosses multiple cul- 
tural registers. 

Yoga's popularity has also prompted numerous 
scholarly studies. These trace the complex routes 
yoga has traveled and the ways its associations with 
Indian culture have been deployed, adapted, and 
imagined (Singleton 2010; Love 2010; White 2009; 
Singleton and Byrne 2008; De Michelis 2004; Alter 
2004; Symon 2011). Parallel to this culturally- 
oriented research, experimental research corrobo- 
rates many of the anecdotal benefits to physical and 
mental health that draw many people to yoga 
(Hasselle-Newcombe 2005:311-2). In particular, the 
calming effects people experience suggest the effect of 
yoga practice on the autonomic nervous system, with 
therapeutic applications ranging from reducing in- 

48 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



flammation of blood vessels to relieving depression 
and extending lifespan (Streeter et al. 2012). The 
absorption of yoga into psychology raises concerns 
about the universalizing tendencies that posit the 
human mind as a cross-cultural constant. 

Yoga is big business and its commercial success 
has prompted critiques of yoga's failure to affect 
Western materialism (Philip 201 1). Yoga offers a 'pa- 
cific spirituality' in the image of ancient Eastern cul- 
tures, which is projected back into Western minds 
and bodies (Bender & McRoberts 2012:14). Critical 
studies investigate how the quest to de-stress the mind 
and sculpt the body serves the goals of consumer 
capitalism, and study how consumerism drives the 
branding of yoga styles, the corporatization of yoga 
studios, and yoga-related products from mats to jew- 
elry (Philip 201 1). 

Yoga as Experiential Practice 

The complexities of cross-cultural transmission and 
trans-religious syncretism continue to motivate criti- 
cal inquiry into how the image of an exotic, passive 
East shapes the appeal of yoga in a marketplace of 
easy spirituality (Carette & King 2005:1 19). However, 
as Paul Heelas observes, 'it could well be the case that 
what is taking place within a yoga group, for exam- 
ple, is too 'rich' for the language of consumption to 
handle' (2008:98). What is taking place may also ex- 
ceed taxonomies that situate yoga (and similar prac- 
tices) only in social and economic domains. While all 
experience is arguably constructed by social envi- 
ronments and discourses (such as 'lifestyle' or 
'health'), inquiring directly into experience may yield 
potent reformulations of the boundaries those tax- 
onomies proscribe. 

Yoga's place in the 'subjective turn' of the early 
2 1 st century is less well researched than its social, cul- 
tural, and therapeutic aspects (Partridge 2005). How- 
ever, its contemplative potential remains vital to the 
experiences cultivated in its practice (Richards 2010). 
Suzanne Hasselle-Newcombe's (2005) sociological 
study of Iyengar style yoga in Britain suggests that for 
the majority of practitioners doing yoga constitutes 'a 
unique act of withdrawing energy from work and 
family responsibilities' (311). This experience of in- 
wardness can be taken as evidence of the focus on 
the self that characterizes a modern 'culture of well- 
being' (Heelas 2008). Yet even in its recontextualiza- 
tion from an ancient ascetic tradition to a modern 
social phenomenon, yoga seems to retain its function 
as 'a distinctive space for introversion and reflection' 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



(Hasselle-Newcombe 2005:311). Against the de- 
mands for self-presentation, socializing, and rapid 
cognition that characterize modern, technologically- 
oriented cultures, yoga identifies interiority as a spe- 
cial kind of experience, and cultivates it. 

Maria Kapsali calls for a theoretical framework 
'that could take into account both the various ideolo- 
gies within/according to which yoga currently oper- 
ates, as well as the personal psychophysical experi- 
ences that practitioners may have' (2012:176). Amid 
the appropriations and distortions wrought by yoga's 
situation in Western cultural norms, how do the ex- 
periences cultivated in its practice press against nor- 
malizing frameworks for experience such as psychol- 
ogy, consumerism, and therapeutics? Psychology may 
be the most familiar model for inner experience in 
modern Western cultures. However, whereas psy- 
chology processes experience through language, yoga 
reverses that direction by cultivating experience at the 
intersection of physical and conceptual processes. By 
what specific processes do practices like modern yoga 
intertwine people's bodies and minds in and as expe- 
rience? To what extent are the tools of consciousness, 
especially language, part of experience rather than 
mediating experience? What methods might pursue 
these questions, and what knowledge would such in- 
vestigation yield? 

Entering Experience 

Critical inquiry relies on a cerebral orientation to 
experience. The question of experience may require 
a bodily-engaged approach. 'We must not assume 
cognition to be purely internal, symbolic, computa- 
tional, and disembodied,' writes cognitive scientist 
Raymond Gibbs, 'but seek out the gross and detailed 
ways that language and thought are inextricably 
shaped by embodied action' (2005:9). This porous 
interaction between mental and physical activity con- 
stitutes the corporeal ether of awareness, which peo- 
ple press into the forms (language, performances, vis- 
ual art, practices) that articulate experience. Applied 
to methods of inquiry, this interaction suggests fluid 
movement among first-person descriptive accounts of 
experience, second-person observation of effects of 
experience, and third-person analysis and representa- 
tion. 

Kristy Nabhan-Warren asks scholars investigating 
religious experience to 'turn to their bodies as sources 
of deep knowledge' and 'take embodied ethnography 
seriously when they write up their findings,' without 
privileging 'mind knowledge over body knowledge' 

49 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



(2011:385). Kimerer LaMothe suggests that the lack 
of bodily investment in scholarship hinders ap- 
proaches to religious experience. She advocates for 
critical sensibilities that go beyond reading bodies as 
texts to engage bodies in motion. LaMothe argues, 'in 
order to advance attention to the bodily dimension of 
religious life, scholars must be willing to acknowledge 
the role played by their own bodily lives in the proc- 
ess of studying religion' (2008:574). For anthropolo- 
gist Thomas Csordas, this stance posits embodiment 
as 'an indeterminate methodological field' in which 
bodily experience is 'the existential ground of culture 
and self (2002:219). Csordas calls for suspending 
reliance on either biology or culture as frameworks 
for understanding bodies to take embodiment as 'an 
experiential understanding of being-in-the-world' 
(2002:241). These approaches open space for attend- 
ing to the corporeal, affective, sensory, and imagina- 
tive components of experience in contemporary 
practices. 

Even as current research situates modern yoga's 
popularity in Western cultural norms, (namely con- 
sumerism, psychology, and therapeutics), yoga in 
practice negotiates those norms. The triangulation of 
mind, body, and spirit in contemporary, vernacular 
practices identifies an experience that collapses this 
three-part linguistic formulation. The question is, 
how? Approaching experience as a process does not 
reduce experience to social constructions or physio- 
logical explanation. This approach neither advocates 
for the phenomenological status of experience nor 
denies it. Rather, it seeks the ways practices like mod- 
ern yoga shape the integration of mental, physical, 
and spiritual capacities as registers of experience, and 
identify that integration as a special kind of experi- 
ence. 

Experience in Word and Action 

The metaphor of 'opening the heart center' in Anu- 
sara yoga articulates a particular interplay between 
mental and physical capacities. It joins physical sensa- 
tion to mental receptivity. An 'open heart center' can 
manifest as emotion, a feeling of the physical body 
coming into alignment, heightened awareness or in- 
sight, suspension of judgment, a shift in perception, 
feeling a part of the body previously not available to 
consciousness, telepathic sensitivity to other people, 
feelings of gratitude or compassion. The metaphor 
also identifies 'the heart center' as the experience of a 
self that is not bound by external norms (social roles, 
identity, culturally imposed concepts). 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



The next two sections focus narrowly on the 
process of 'opening the heart center.' In practice, the 
'heart center' structures a bodily experience, which 
gives rise to the descriptive metaphor. However, 
rather than make the body an object of consciousness 
or seek pre -linguistic consciousness, this process reor- 
ganizes people's conceptual, corporeal, and meta- 
physical capacities into registers of experience. A 
close reading of Anusara's Teacher Training Manual 
shows how this metaphor shapes and becomes part of 
corporeal experience. An embodied reading of the 
physicality involved in cultivating the 'open heart 
center' shows the concept and the body intertwined 
in the experience of 'opening the heart center.' 

Close Reading: 
The Metaphor of the Heart Center 

The Manual defines Anusara as a system that aligns 
the physical body with 'a non-dual Tantric philoso- 
phy that is epitomized by a 'celebration of the heart" 
[italics added] (Friend 2009:20). With regard to expe- 
rience, the metaphor of the heart center functions in 
multiple, sometimes contradictory or tautological, 
ways. The heart is variously a container for the self, 
an object that can be experienced, a conduit for ex- 
perience, the site of experience, and experience itself. 
By doing yoga people express their 'specific heart 
qualities or virtues' (Friend 2009:93). The heart cen- 
ter contains three aspects of a person's essence: per- 
sonal moral qualities (modesty or steadfastness), spiri- 
tual ideals (union with the divine; 'Shakti' energy), 
and emotions (anger, love). Postures are a way to 'ex- 
perience joy in the heart' [italics added] (Friend 
2009:93). The heart has 'two fundamental states,' 
which are 'happiness and unhappiness' (Friend 
2009:27). Understanding the postures as 'heart- 
oriented' suggests that the heart center itself becomes 
a sensory organ with access to a particular kind of 
experience, or that the heart becomes its own kind of 
consciousness (Friend 2009:21). The Manual identi- 
fies an experience of being 'in the flow' which 'is 
simply to open our hearts with love to the present 
moment without clinging or pushing' [italics added] 
(Friend 2009:20). These ambiguities blur physical 
feeling with an intellectual concept. 

In practice, body and mind are reciprocal capaci- 
ties. The physicality of the postures opens 'the heart 
center,' but practitioners have agency and responsibil- 
ity. Bodies respond to a disposition of mind ('inten- 
tion' or Attitude'). With the intention to 'soften and 
expand' the heart, a person can 'release or dissolve 

50 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



self-induced [physical and mental] tensions and 
[physical and mental] limitations, and to open to the 
greatness and the tremendous divine potential within' 
(Friend 2009:104). A 'pure spiritual expression from 
the heart,' according to the Manual, gives a posture 
'its power for deep inner transformation' (Friend 
2009:26). That transformational 'power of the heart' 
relies as much on a person's intention as the physical 
effort of the practice. According to the Manual, the 
'power of the heart' is the force behind every action 
or full expression of a posture (Friend 2009:26). 

There is, thus, a paradox built into the metaphor 
of the 'heart center.' The agency that affects the 
heart also comes from the heart. In describing Anu- 
sara yoga as a 'heart-oriented' practice, the Manual 
holds out the potential for 'inner transformational 
power' when the heart center is physically expanded 
in a posture and when a practitioner is mentally re- 
ceptive to the effects of the physical practice (Friend 
2009:106). At the same time, the physical effort that 
holds a body in any posture requires a practitioner's 
'surrender or open-heartedness' for the posture to be 
effective (Friend 2009:28). In this way, the physical 
practice is invested with a metaphysical, quasi- 
spiritual sensation of an open heart (Friend 
2009:101). 

Anusara's metaphor of the 'heart center' strips 
away overtly religious, meditative, or spiritual con- 
texts for yoga, though the valence remains in the lan- 
guage of transformation, spirituality, and metaphysi- 
cal power. Equating the heart with a sense of self has 
an obvious resonance with the popular construction 
of the modern self as a site of transformation and 
healing, with the added ethical imperative of aligning 
the physical body with social, as well as individual, 
wellbeing. The lack of explicit reference to Hindu 
sources in the Manual distances Anusara from its In- 
dian roots. 

Selective references to Hinduism, however, pro- 
vide a conceptual backdrop, they contextualize 'open- 
ing the heart center' as a special experience. Allusions 
to Tantra, Shavism, and Bhakti elide spirituality with 
feeling good as a timeless quality of experience rather 
than specifically Hindu. Traditional Anusara classes, 
for example, begin with chanting in Sanskrit the 
Anusara Yoga Opening Invocation' (Friend 2009:iii). 
The Manual's translation renders the original text's 
reference to Lord Siva in the phrase namah shivaya 
gurave as 'luminous Teacher within and without' 
(Friend 2009:iii). The words come from the Nira- 
lamba Upanishad, a minor or general Upanishad 
from the Sukla Yajur Veda (Pradeep 2008: 124). What 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



Anusara practitioners chant is half of the first short 
section of the Upanishad, and not included in most 
translations. The words, intended for people on a 
renunciant yogic path who lack a guru, introduce 
forty-one questions about the nature of Brahman. 
This excerpt out of context is inconsistent with Anu- 
sara's purportedly Tantric emphasis on non-dual em- 
bodiment. In my observation, however, the inconsis- 
tency is not important to practitioners. Rather, the 
words function as inspiration to pursue self knowl- 
edge through the yoga practice. 

The metaphor of the 'heart center' raises the 
spectre of exoticization and the problem of perenni- 
alism. Though they are not studied in the practice, 
Anusara's identification of the heart center as the site 
of self knowledge resonates with traditions in classi- 
cal yoga. Compilations of source texts, as well as 
Patanjali's Yoga Sutra (200 BCE) and the Hatha Yoga 
Pradipika (14th c. CE), are readily available in Eng- 
lish translations often sold in studios. Most prominent 
are those by George Feuerstein (1947-2012), a practi- 
tioner and translator based in Canada who grounded 
his yoga teaching in Hindu traditions, and Mircea 
Eliade (1907-1986), whose scholarship at the Univer- 
sity of Chicago mapped religious experience as an 
academic field. These compilations do not require a 
specialists' knowledge Sanskrit or Hindu traditions, or 
the ability to parse Vedic thought, Tantra, Shaivism, 
Vaishanavism, and Bhakti as historical developments. 
Drawing indirectly on these complex philosophical 
traditions and distilling them into a simplistic formu- 
lation, Anusara's metaphor of the 'heart center' 
shapes the aspirations of the practice as experiential, 
not intellectual: a felt sense emanating from and ani- 
mating the center of the chest. 

Embodied Reading: 
Physical Movement and 
The Heart Center 

The body is the site for a register of experience that is 
simultaneously conceptual and corporeal. Anusara's 
approach is biomechanical. The 'heart center' refers 
not to the heart muscle but the center of the chest 
between the sternum and the spine, the mediastinum 
where the bones and muscles of the ribcage encase 
the organs of oxygen exchange (Abrahams 
2010:186-87). The bottom of this physical area, 
roughly the lateral plane of the relaxed diaphragm, is 
considered a place of physical and psychic or spiritual 
power. A practitioner is taught to draw kinesthetic 
energy inward toward this center and radiate non- 

51 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



physical energy outward as the body moves into a 
posture (Friend 2009:31). Practitioners learn to 'sof- 
ten' or 'melt' the heart center from within the body 
and 'open' or 'lift' the heart center. These are the 
subtle, internal moves that may prompt release of 
emotions, relaxing of mental concepts, feelings of 
receptivity, or a sense of receptivity. 

Mechanical actions broaden the ribcage, which 
creates the physiological context for internally melt- 
ing, softening, lifting, and opening the heart center as 
well as for sensing the heartbeat and breath. This 
process of moving consciousness into the body as the 
body emerges as a kind of consciousness has the po- 
tential to shift practitioners' conventional organiza- 
tion of self as mind and body. The 'heart center' 
takes on the capacity for consciousness, which con- 
ventionally belongs to the conceptualizing, narrative 
capacities of the mind. An embodied reading of how 
one posture works cannot take us directly into peo- 
ple's experience, but it can illustrate one process by 
which this yoga practice identifies 'opening the heart 
center' as special for practitioners. 

The backbend is familiar to gymnasts, dancers, 
and other athletic performers as well as to yoga prac- 
titioners. Yoga names this basic posture 'upward bow' 
or Udhrva Dhanurasana. From a supine position, the 
feet and hands press the chest, abdomen, and hips 
upward, into the shape of a bow with the floor as the 
bow's taut string. Beyond the mindful engagement 
with the body required to hold the posture, the meta- 
phor of the 'heart center' organizes the integration of 
mental and physical capacities, which distinguishes 
yoga's 'upward bow' from a contortionist's or gym- 
nast's backbend. 

'Upward bow' can produce a range of sensatVions 
that might be experienced as revealing a deep and 
profound part of the self. Practitioners are taught to 
pull the heads of the upper armbones (humerus) into 
the shoulder sockets toward the heart center. In an 
upside down orientation, working against gravity, this 
kinesthetic action can release the upper palate in re- 
sponse to the pull of the trapezius muscles. One re- 
sponse to this physical change can be a relaxation in 
the forehead, or a sense of light in the region of the 
light-regulating pineal gland and which the yogic 
chakra system identifies as the 'third eye' (Maxwell 
2009). 'Upward bow' pushes the thoracic vertebrae, 
which are the least amenable to flexion toward the 
sternum, and extends the top of the sternum through 
the shoulders. This action can release the internal 
intercostals, which are less accessible to conscious 
control than the back, arms and legs. This unfamiliar 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



release can produce a shift in awareness (Kaminof 
2007:203). 

'Upward bow' also disrupts the familiar auto- 
nomic patterns of breathing. It expands the medi- 
asternum front to back and side to side, changing the 
familiar feeling of expanding with an inhalation and 
contracting on an exhalation. With the top of the 
head parallel to the floor and the ribcage inverted, 
the flow of the breath feels reversed — inhaling moves 
the breath up (toward the ceiling) rather than in the 
familiar downward direction. Extending the spine 
moves the ribs farther apart, even as exhalation draws 
the ribs together (Coulter 2001:281). This position 
also orients the eyes so that no part of the body may 
be visible, disorienting familiar perceptual patterns 
and the ability to conceptually orient the environ- 
ment around the body which can prompt shifts in 
perception. Extending the psoas muscle, the longest 
muscle of the body running from the bottom of the 
thorax through the pelvis to the thighs, can have the 
effect of releasing tension in the organs protected by 
the rib cage and yield a sense of the entire 'heart cen- 
ter' relaxing, metaphorically 'opening' 

Postures that open the front of the thorax and 
shoulders are particularly likely to generate experi- 
ences such as sudden rushes of emotion, and the 
Teacher Training Manual gives guidance for dealing 
with emotional responses to postures (Friend 
2009:80). Whether or not practitioners connect these 
experiences to traditions of the heart as a source of 
illumination described in source texts on yogic medi- 
tation, in this practice the 'heart center' takes on ex- 
periential significance in this practice in a language of 
inner transformation, celebration, knowing a pure or 
true self, and openness to divinity. 

Opening Experience 

In numerous vernacular practices — yoga, t'ai chi, 
contemplative dance, martial arts, shamanic drum- 
ming are but a few — people cultivate registers of ex- 
perience that are neither neurologically top-down nor 
bottom-up and which do not conform to conven- 
tional organizations of experience as mental or bod- 
ily activity. A process approach to these experiences 
opens a range of questions into the subtleties of ex- 
perience. How do practices differ in the ways they 
constitute mind and body interaction? Modern yoga, 
the example presented here, has been analyzed for its 
appropriation of Indian religious culture and history 
even as yoga is critiqued for its investment in Western 
culture. How do those cultural and discursive influ- 

52 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



ences shape experiences as special? When does expe- 
rience resist those shapes and create new shapes? 

There are always gaps between the social and 
economic contexts that proscribe meaning and the 
internal sensations people experience. What is the 
process between the act of putting the body in a yoga 
posture and identifying the experience of an illumi- 
nating 'opening'? How do special experiences such as 
'opening the heart center' become the basis for 
knowledge, social interaction, discourses, and institu- 
tional organizations? How might special experiences 
morph into ideologies, possibly transforming libera- 
tory experiences into mechanisms of control? How 
do these experiences challenge models in which the 
mind acts on the body or the body influences mental 
capacities? 

Finally, what epistemological queries might such 
practices be making? David Maxwell asserts that 
there is 'valid truth in the experience of yogis that 
can provide important information concerning com- 
plex aspects of our human condition' (2009:819). 
The experience of an 'open heart center,' for exam- 
ple, like the proposition of a 'heart charkra,' asks how 
non-physical properties (for Anusara 'Shakti energy') 
interact with physical bodies (Maxwell 2009:809). 
What might such a proposition offer taxonomies that 
distinguish mental from physical activity, or between 
scholarship and embodied practices? In what ways 
does language participate in experience, rather drive 
a wedge of critical distance? 

The intertwining of corporeality and conscious- 
ness in the construction of knowledge so prominent 
in vernacular practices distorts the boundary between 
constructing knowledge from observation and deriv- 
ing knowledge from experience. Maria Kapsali sum- 
marizes the challenge this distortion presents specifi- 
cally with regard to Modern Postural Yoga (MPY): 
'the number of discourses, psychophysical possibili- 
ties, and metaphysical expectations that operate not 
only within the space of practice, but in a number of 
similar disciplines, point toward a need to reconfigure 
these terms that MPY loves to employ but hates to 
define' (2012:176). Observation and practice meet in 
a reciprocal desire for knowledge of experience and 
the experience of knowledge. The task now is to bet- 
ter understand the processes that constitute special 
experiences and embrace the challenges these experi- 
ential practices might pose to conventional models of 
cultural analysis. 



Biography 




Donnalee Dox received 
her Ph.D. from the Uni- 
versity of Minnesota in 
1995. Her primary re- 
search area is European 
theatre in the 1 0th-1 5th 
centuries, and her sec- 
ondary area is contem- 
porary religious perform- 
ance practices. Dr. Dox 
has published on music, 
dance and theatre in 
journals such as Theatre 
History Studies, Theatre 

Research International, Frontiers: A Journal of Woman 
Studies, and The Journal of Dramatic Theatre and Criti- 
cism. She has presented papers at numerous medieval 
conferences, including Convivium, the Arizona Center for 
Medieval and Renaissance Studies, the Medieval Acad- 
emy, and the International Congress on Medieval Studies. 
In the Department of Performance Studies, she teaches 
script analysis and dramaturgy, and is developing new in- 
terdisciplinary courses. 



New Publication of Interest: 
The Devil Within: Possession and 
Exorcism in the Christian West 
Brian P. Levack 






Vol. 4 No. 3 



53 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



The Brain and Spiritual Experiences: 
Towards a Neuroscientific Hermeneutic 

Andrew B. Newberg 




Spiritual experiences are fundamental to the human 
interpretation of the world around us. Historically, 
spiritual experiences appear to have laid the founda- 
tion for religious belief systems, as well as moral, po- 
litical, and cultural systems. In the past twenty years, 
an expanding amount of research into the brain 
mechanisms of spiritual experiences has provided 
scholars with new information about the nature of 
these experiences. But this research also continues to 
propel us towards the development of a neuroepiste- 
mological perspective on human experience, and par- 
ticularly the experience of reality. Hybrid fields such 
as neurotheology have arisen in order to better assess 
the implications of studying spiritual experiences 
from a neuroscientific perspective. Neurotheology 
argues that combining science with religious and 
spiritual phenomena can provide a variety of new 
approaches to the study of human experience and its 
implications for our ability to understand reality. This 
might lead to a parallel philosophical field that could 
be referred to as a neuroscientific hermeneutics. Such 
a neurohermeneutics argues that human experience, 
including spiritual experience, is highly influenced, 
although not necessarily caused, by the brain. Thus, 
developing an understanding of the relationship be- 
tween the brain and subjective experience will help us 
to better understand how we interpret experiences, 
make meaning, and relate that meaning through ab- 
stract thought and language. 

This paper presents an overview of some of the 
research on spiritual experiences that myself and 
other colleagues have been pursuing over the past 
two decades. But, more importantly, this paper will 
also consider a neurohermeneutical approach to hu- 
man experience. We will reflect on the ontological 
status of human subjective experience in general, and 
spiritual experiences in particular. It must be noted at 
the outset that while some of this discussion will ap- 
pear to involve a materialistic reductionism, the im- 
plications of such a methodology, in the light of neu- 
rohermeneutics and neuroepistemology leaves us 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



with an ontology that is anything but reductionistic. 
These considerations, however, must await the con- 
clusion of this paper. 

The Neuropsychology 
of Spiritual Experience 

There is clearly a wide variety of spiritual experi- 
ences. Ninian Smart distinguished between the expe- 
rience of Rudolf Otto's 'wholly other' and the inter- 
nal sense of ineffable unity defined as a mystical ex- 
perience, predominantly although not exclusively, in 
Asian traditions. Ninian Smart (1969; 1978) has ar- 
gued that certain sects of Hinduism, Buddhism, and 
Taoism differ markedly from prophetic religions such 
as Judaism and Islam and from religions related to 
the prophetic -like Christianity, in that the religious 
experience most characteristic of the former is 'mys- 
tical' whereas that most characteristic of the latter is 
'numinous.' Of these two terms, it is the numinous 
that Smart seems to have an easier time explaining 
since it obviously arises more spontaneously out of 
Western religious traditions. W.T Stace (1961) went 
further by distinguishing between what he calls extro- 
vertive mystical experiences and introvertive mystical 
experiences. Extrovertive mystical experiences are 
distinguished by a unifying vision in which all things 
are perceived as one and there is an apprehension of 
the One as an inner subjectivity or life in all things. 
Introvertive mystical experiences are distinguished by 
including a unitary consciousness or awareness which 
is nonspatial and nontemporal. 

With regard to the continuum of spiritual experi- 
ences, and based upon a neuropsychological analysis, 
unitary states appear to play a crucial role and we will 
consider this in more detail below. While it is difficult 
to define what makes a given experience 'spiritual,' 
the sense of having a union with some higher power 
or ultimate reality seems a crucial part of spiritual 
experiences. To that end, this union helps reduce ex- 
istential anxiety as well as provide a sense of control 
over the environment (d'Aquili 1978; 1998). Impor- 

54 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



tantly, describing the phenomenology of subjective 
spiritual experiences includes a sense of the unity of 
reality that is at least somewhat greater than the base- 
line perception of unity in day to day life (d'Aquili 
1986). This may be related to altered functioning of 
the brain structures typically involved in helping to 
construct the self/ other dichotomy Usually the self/ 
other dichotomy functions to help us distinguish our 
self from the rest of the external world. I have sug- 
gested that the left superior parietal lobe may play a 
critical role in establishing the self/other dichotomy 
Joseph 1996). This dichotomy is normally based 
upon input from all of the sensory systems. In cases 
of meditation, my colleagues and I have suggested, 
and found brain imaging evidence for, a differential 
blocking, or 'deafferentation,' of input into the supe- 
rior parietal lobe which progressively diminishes the 
strength of the self/other dichotomy (d'Aquili and 
Newberg 1993; Newberg et al. 2001). Thus, if the 
continuum of spiritual experience heavily relies on 
the progressive sense of unity, this should be associ- 
ated with progressive blocking of input into the supe- 
rior parietal lobe. Such a physiological change should 
be associated with an increasing sense of unity over 
multiplicity. In addition, the right superior parietal 
lobe is involved in orienting ourselves within three 
dimensional space (Newberg 2010). The blocking of 
sensory input into this structure may result in the al- 
terations in the sense of space and time that are often 
described during spiritual experiences. Thus, both the 
left and right superior parietal lobes are likely in- 
volved in spiritual experiences. 

At the extreme end of the continuum of spiritual 
experiences is the state of Absolute Unitary Being 
which is described in the mystical literature of all the 
world's great religions (d'Aquili and Newberg 1999). 
When a person is in that state he or she loses all sense 
of discrete being and even the difference between self 
and other is obliterated. There is no sense of the 
passing of time, and all that remains is a perfect time- 
less undifferentiated consciousness. When such a state 
is suffused with positive affect (i.e. emotional content), 
there is a tendency to describe the experience, after 
the fact, as personal. Such experiences are often de- 
scribed as a perfect union with God (the Unio mys- 
tica of the Christian tradition), or else the perfect 
manifestation of God in the Hindu tradition. When 
such experiences are accompanied by neutral affect 
they tend to be described, after the fact, as imper- 
sonal. These states are described in concepts such as 
the abyss of Jacob Boeme, the Void or Nirvana of 
Buddhism or the Absolute of a number of 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



philosophical/mystical traditions. There is no ques- 
tion that whether the experience is interpreted per- 
sonally as God or impersonally as the Absolute, it 
nevertheless possesses a quality of transcendent 
wholeness without any temporal or spatial division 
whatsoever. We have postulated that these rare states 
of Absolute Unitary Being are associated with the 
total blocking of input into the superior parietal lobe 
(d'Aquili and Newberg 1993). 

Due to connections between the superior parietal 
lobe and the limbic system, the structures that sub- 
serve emotional responses, it may be that the more 
blocking of input into the right superior parietal lobe, 
the stronger will be the associated emotional dis- 
charge in the limbic system. The involvement of the 
limbic system in such experiences is supported by 
studies in which electrical stimulation of two limbic 
structures, the amygdala and hippocampus, has re- 
sulted in various sensory experiences, visions, and 
emotional discharges similar to some of those that 
occur during spiritual experiences (Penfield and Perot 
1963; Valenstein 1973). Limbic stimulation during 
spiritual experiences may be modulated by activity in 
the superior parietal lobes as well as the frontal lobes 
since these structures are all intimately intercon- 
nected Joseph 1996). During practices such as medi- 
tation, stimulation of the limbic system may result 
from activity in the frontal cortex which is known to 
modulate emotional responses through its connec- 
tions with the amygdala and hippocampus. Increased 
frontal lobe activity has been shown to occur during 
meditation and likely occurs during other types of 
spiritual practices (Lazar et al. 2000; Newberg et al. 
2001; Herzog et al. 1990-1991). Thus, in any percep- 
tion, such as a piece of music, a painting, a sculpture, 
or a sunset, there is a sense of meaning and whole- 
ness which transcends the constituent parts. 

In aesthetic experiences such as those just de- 
scribed, this transcendence is mild to moderate. The 
overarching sense of unity between two persons in 
romantic love might represent the next stage in this 
continuum. Feelings of numinosity or religious awe 
occur when there is a very marked sense of meaning 
and wholeness extending well beyond the parts per- 
ceived, or well beyond the image generated, but in a 
'wholly other' context. It is often considered (al- 
though not necessarily correctly) to be the dominant 
Western mystical experience. It is experienced when 
an archetypal symbol is perceived, or when certain 
archetypal elements are externally constellated in a 
myth. As one moves from numinosity along the con- 
tinuum, one can reach the state of religious exalta- 

55 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



tion characterized by a sense of meaning and whole- 
ness extending to all discrete being whether subjective 
or objective (d'Aquili and Newberg 1999). The essen- 
tial unity and purposefulness of the universe is per- 
ceived as a primary datum despite the perception and 
knowledge of evil in the world. During this state, 
there is nothing whatsoever that escapes the mantle of 
wholeness and purposefulness. But this state does not 
obliterate discrete being, and it certainly exists within 
a temporal context. This roughly corresponds to 
Stace's extrovertive mystical experience. 

From a neurohemeneutical perspective, how can 
we begin to interpret all of these different experiences 
and their meaning? It would be helpful to determine 
if there are particular neuropsychological constructs 
that help towards understanding these experiences. 
Furthermore, it will be helpful to utilize this informa- 
tion in a way that contributes something to episte- 
mology. Let us explore in more detail how we might 
be able to construct a neurohermeneutical approach 
towards spiritual experiences. 

The Beginnings of 
a Neurohermeneutic 

In the above section, we have begun to see how brain 
functions might be related to specific components of 
spiritual experiences such as unity or emotions. This 
raises the larger consideration that the brain affects 
all of our perceptions, thoughts, and feelings. To 
some degree, the brain may augment or create our 
experiences, thoughts, and feelings, and to some de- 
gree, the brain may receive them. Furthermore, 
whether causing or experiencing our world, the brain 
likely restricts or constrains the ways in which we ex- 
perience the world. Thus, when applied to philoso- 
phy and epistemology, such an analysis can lead to a 
new hermeneutical approach in which we consider 
the influence of the brain on a variety of ideological 
positions. It would seem appropriate to consider this 
approach as a neurohermeneutic - how the brain 
influences human experiences and ideas about reality 
(Newberg 2010). It should be clear though, that what 
we are exploring through this neurohermeneutical 
analysis is how a given individual experiences some 
aspect of neuropsychological function, which ulti- 
mately is associated with a specific idea or concept 
about the universe. In essence then, we are construct- 
ing a neurohermeneutic regarding how the brain is 
related to our experiences and how these experiences 
affect, alter, and constrain the human ability to think 
specific theological and philosophical thoughts. We 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



are also developing, in some regard, a new philo- 
sophical system which might be called 'experiential- 
ist' such that all thinking, emotions, and ideas, are 
tied to human experience. This is akin to the Kantian 
position that the external world is only known to us 
through our perceptions and ideas. However, neu- 
rotheology has the potential to take this notion fur- 
ther since 'experience' in this context does not refer 
only to sensory experience, but the experience of our 
own internal cognitive, emotional, and perhaps, spiri- 
tual processes. Finally, a neurohermeneutic also offers 
the potential for obtaining empirical data to support 
or refute specific ideas. 

In addition to empirical data that might be ob- 
tained through some scientific method, neuroherme- 
neutics argues for obtaining the equally important 
data from subjective experience. This might not be 
too dissimilar from Husserl's phenomenology, but 
certain distinctions should be identified as we proceed 
through this neurohermeneutical analysis. The pur- 
pose of our experientialist analysis is to determine 
exactly what parts of the human being allow us to 
have experiences so that we may understand the sub- 
jective nature of the experiences as ascertained 
through a phenomenological analysis. In this way, 
neurohermeneutics might actually be a blending of 
Kantian philosophy with phenomenology. As we will 
see, such an analysis may have profound implications 
for theological and philosophical thought, hermeneu- 
tics itself, and phenomenology. This will be particu- 
larly the case in the analysis of epistemological issues 
pertaining to the experience of reality and the identi- 
fication of the characteristics by which we define real- 

ity ' 

A neurohermeneutical approach argues that we 
should strive to understand all of reality from the 
cognitive, emotional, and perceptual processes associ- 
ated with the brain. But, if neurohermeneutics is to 
be a true hybrid approach, there should be a compa- 
rable contribution of science and various aspects of 
human experience, including spiritual experiences. 
Thus, a neurohermeneutic must recognize that this 
approach may prove useful for understanding the 
basis of the scientific disciplines as well as religious 
ones. Can we not ask why science has developed in 
the current manner? How much of science is based 
upon what makes sense to our brain? How much of 
science is based upon the ways in which the brain has 
shaped the ways in which we conceive of the reality 
of the world? This suggests a need to more clearly 
determine how the brain perceives reality utilizing 
this neurohermeneutical approach. 

56 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Spiritual Experiences 
as Primary Epistemic States 

When evaluating how we come to know the external 
world, we must begin with how we come to know 
anything at all. A neurohermeneutical approach 
would acknowledge that the only way in which hu- 
man beings come to know what is real is through the 
various senses and the brain's processing of that sen- 
sory input. The brain takes all of the sensory input, 
utilizes its cognitive and emotional resources, and 
puts together a 'rendition' of the world with which an 
individual can interact. Outward actions or behaviors 
then have consequences in the world that are per- 
ceived in addition to whatever was already out there 
in the external world. The external world is what is 
actually objectively real regardless of human percep- 
tions and cognitions. It would seem almost impossible 
to completely get at what is ultimately, objectively real 
because any information or sense that is received 
about this objective reality necessarily must come 
through the human brain. 

But we must now ask another question — Why 
does something feel real to us at all? In other words, 
when we perceive a table, listen to someone talk to us, 
or even have a spiritual experience, we have the 
strong tendency to perceive these things as real. Is the 
sense that something is real based upon perceptions 
only, consistency of time, emotions, logic? Regardless, 
neurohermeneutics would also remind us that how- 
ever the brain comes to perceive something as real 
has no clear bearing on what is actually, absolutely 
real, but rather relates to our experience of whatever 
is real. This issue will be addressed more at the end of 
this article. For now, though, let us explore how the 
brain does experience reality and more specifically, 
how it informs us what it thinks is real. At this point 
then, we are forced to explore only the sense of real- 
ity that is created for us by the brain. We have noth- 
ing more to go on, at least yet. 

In fact, we may find ourselves contemplating the 
notion that what we use to assess if something is real 
ultimately comes down to our profound sense that it 
is real. This is certainly not a very satisfying conclu- 
sion. But from a neurohermeneutical perspective, we 
may find no other clear way to assess how real some- 
thing is. It seems that any criteria we might use is ul- 
timately reducible to our sense that it is real. Typi- 
cally, we cite criteria such as vividness, persistence, 
cross reference, logicalness, or any other criteria, they 
all seem to collapse into the sense of realness. After 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



all, each criteria represents some aspect of reality that 
we also must sense. Thus, vividness refers to a clarity 
of perception. But a perception is also sensed as be- 
ing real. If we perceive the persistence of an object 
over time, that too is a qualia that requires our sense 
that the persistence itself is real. And if we ask for 
cross referencing with other individuals, their re- 
sponses are sensed as being real. How do we know 
which of these senses of reality are actually real? We 
have no way of knowing other than by trying to as- 
sess the strength of the sense that something is real. 
Again, though, this is not necessarily comforting epis- 
temologically since it does not tell us what external 
reality actually is like. We are trapped within our 
brain peering out into the world and reconstructing it 
the best we can. We inherently experience a 'second- 
hand' rendition of the world. 

A substantial additional concern regarding our 
reliance on the sense of realness we experience is that 
this approach might result in relativism or solipsism. 
After all, if everything in reality is at best experienced 
as a perception by the brain, then there might be no 
absolute. Or at least, there is no absolute that our 
brain can determine. In addition, it must always be 
remembered that the perceptions of reality and real- 
ity itself are not necessarily commensurate. Relativ- 
ism might apply to human perceptions, but it does 
not necessarily apply to actual reality Similarly, solip- 
sism would suggest that the self is the only reality and 
the self is the only thing that can be known. While 
these notions might be true on one hand, they too are 
perceptions of reality and thus, even a solipsistic 
stance must be regarded as a perception of the brain 
in much the same way as any other experience of 
reality. 

Can there be some way around this paradoxical 
problem in which there is a fundamental disconnect 
between our perceptions of reality and actual reality? 
Although such a problem might be unresolvable, 
neurohermeneutics would suggest that we should be- 
gin by exploring our perceptions of reality since we 
have no choice but to begin here. In the reality that 
we perceive on a daily basis, what might be called 
'everyday' or 'baseline' reality, there is a very strong 
sense that what is perceived is, in fact, real. One 
might call this sense of reality a Primary Epistemic 
State of the brain (Newberg 2010). It should be men- 
tioned that such a state is to some extent a brain state 
and to some extent a phenomenological state. It is the 
brain that enables that experiential state that subse- 
quently enables an individual to perceive that experi- 
ence as real. The primary epistemic state of baseline 

57 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



reality, however, is only one way in which the brain 
can perceive reality. Thus, there may be a number of 
epistemic states. Further, these states might be con- 
sidered 'primary' because they are not derived from 
sense perception per se, but rather define the form 
and understanding of that perception. Theoretically, 
they also would not be reducible into each other. 

Why are primary epistemic states important to 
spiritual experiences and the development of neuro- 
hermeneutics? One of the most important points is 
that understanding such states will be crucial for 
helping develop a nomenclature for various religious 
and spiritual states, particularly mystical ones. It is in 
these spiritual or mystical states that individuals often 
recount the realness of the experience, and the divine 
or absolute nature of the experience. For many, such 
an experience lies at the heart of their religious or 
spiritual expression. Furthermore, an epistemological 
analysis of different epistemic states might be crucial 
for determining which view — scientific, religious, or 
otherwise - has the best perspective on the true na- 
ture of reality even if distinguishing our perception of 
reality from reality itself is a difficult task. 

What makes any primary epistemic state define 
reality for a particular person is the individual's sense, 
when they are in one of these states, that what they 
are experiencing is fundamentally or ultimately real. 
This is a crucial aspect since it would seem essential 
that when one is in a primary epistemic state, it is 
perceived as if that state represents what is actually 
real. Once the person leaves a state and settles into a 
second one, they typically perceive the original state 
to no longer represent actual reality. In this case, any 
other perception of reality is considered to be an illu- 
sion or deception. Other than baseline reality, the 
other epistemic state that most people are familiar 
with is dreams. During a dream, everything that is 
experienced is usually treated as real even when 
things do not follow logical ordering or do not appear 
vivid. The point here is that a dream is perceived to 
be real during the dream, and then recognized as 
'just a dream' upon awakening. Once back in base- 
line reality, there is the perception that the dream 
state, or any other for that matter, does not represent 
actual reality. 

In order to determine what is really real and the 
characteristics of these primary epistemic states, neu- 
rohermeneutics can attempt to derive the nature of 
these states based upon both human experience and 
the functioning of the brain. A neurohermeneutical 
approach should typically include several important 
elements with regard to primary epistemic states. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



These elements are determined primarily by how 
human beings sense and make sense of reality. This 
requires sensory elements, cognitive elements, and 
emotional elements. In fact, it might be helpful to 
break down the primary epistemic state into three 
parameters, each of which relate not only to the ex- 
perience but to brain functions (Newberg 2011): (1) 
perceptions of objects or beings which can be mani- 
fested as either multiple discrete things (i.e. more than 
one), or as a holistic union of all things (a unitary re- 
ality in which everything is one), (2) relationships be- 
tween objects or things that are either regular or ir- 
regular, and (3) emotional responses to the objects or 
things that are either positive, negative, or neutral. 

Each of these parameters is well known to our 
own perceptions of the world. Human beings appear 
to only perceive the world as consisting of either mul- 
tiple discrete objects or as a unity. We are born with 
the neurological capability to observe, name, and 
manipulate multiple objects as discrete things. The 
abstract and reductionist processes of the brain help 
in that regard. Language too is essential in labeling 
objects and categorizing them. Thus, we distinguish 
between a spruce tree, a mountain, and a dog. We 
have extensive nomenclature for naming animals and 
plants, atoms and molecules, and ethical and religious 
frameworks. The areas of the brain involved in cate- 
gorization and naming have been studied in the field 
of cognitive neuroscience and lend support to the 
importance of these structures and their associated 
functions in establishing our perceptions of reality. 

If there is the perception that there are absolutely 
no discrete objects, the person experiences the state of 
absolute unity which we considered above. There 
may be a variety of states with an increasing sense of 
unification of things, but philosophically speaking, it 
would seem that there could only be one state in 
which there is a complete and absolute unity of all 
things. This experience includes the sense that the 
individual is part of the unity such that there is no 
self and no other. Otherwise, there would be discrete 
objects, namely the self and the other. Evidence from 
brain imaging supports the notion that parts of the 
brain that typically integrate sensory information into 
a sense of self and an orientation of that self with 
respect to the world might be affected during spiritual 
practices that lead to unitary states (Newberg et al. 
2001). However, it may be impossible to scientifically 
measure the changes associated with absolute unitary 
states since there is no clear way to know when some- 
one else is actually in such a state. It is impossible for 
an individual to report that they are having an expe- 

58 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



rience of absolute unity, it is likely that it will never be 
known what pattern of brain activity is associated 
with this experience. 

In our perceptions of the world, there are also 
important causal and logical relationships between 
the objects we perceive in the world. When such rela- 
tionships appear to make sense to us, we refer to 
them as regular. The causal processes of the brain 
play a critical role in the ability to evaluate relation- 
ships between objects and triggers a response in us 
when unexpected things occur. When causality seems 
disrupted, we experience an emotional response that 
alerts us to the disruption. When relationships are 
irregular, we note that they do not appear to follow 
established pathways based upon our prior experi- 
ences of reality. Research on infants to adults shows 
that we respond differently, and activate different 
parts of the brain, when confronted with irregular 
relationships whether they are grammatical, musical, 
logical, or any other type of relationship. 

Emotional responses (or affect) in humans are far 
ranging in their composition. However, they appear 
to eventually be classified into three broad categories 
- positive, negative, and neutral. Positive emotions 
include happiness, joy, elation, love, and contentment. 
Negative emotions include fear, sadness, depression, 
anxiety, anger, and melancholy. The absence of ei- 
ther positive or negative emotions would be catego- 
rized as neutral. Many cognitive neuroscience studies 
have evaluated how the brain processes positive and 
negative emotions with the realization that emotions 
can be compared to a neutral state. The emotional 
responses in primary epistemic states, however, do 
not refer to the usual feelings of happiness and sad- 
ness that most people refer to, but to the overall emo- 
tional approach of the person to their reality. In other 
words, the whole world is viewed as positive or nega- 
tive rather than feeling positive at some points and 
negative at others. 

It is also important to mention that each of these 
parameters is most likely set along a continuum. 
Thus, one may have an experience of reality that is 
based primarily on having multiple discrete objects, 
but may also have some unitary attributes. Similarly, 
there may be some regular and some irregular rela- 
tionships between objects. However, this notation al- 
lows for an overall perspective from which more spe- 
cific elements of primary epistemic states can be 
elaborated. Based upon these parameters there ap- 
pear to be nine possible primary epistemic states that 
are internally consistent, and should have neurologi- 
cal and phenomenological correlates. It should also 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



be noted that an individual might enter into many 
different states during their lifetime. They may re- 
main in one state briefly, for many years, or for their 
entire life. But they might also shift from one primary 
epistemic state to another, and sometimes quite fre- 
quently. 

As an example, in addition to the primary epis- 
temic state of everyday reality, there are other states 
such as dream states, psychotic states, and drug in- 
duced states that contain multiple discrete objects 
that we appear to respond to. And each of these 
states may include positive, negative, or neutral emo- 
tions. Importantly, when an individual is in one of 
these states, they act as if that state is the true reality 
at the moment. For example, when a person is 
dreaming, their reactions to the events in the dream 
do not take into account everyday reality. The person 
responds as if the dream state represents the real real- 
ity. It is only upon awakening from the dream that we 
relegate it to an inferior perception of reality and 
now treat everyday reality as the real reality. This is 
true of each of these epistemic states in which there 
are discrete objects. This is also true for states in 
which there is a progressive sense of the unity of all 
things. This can include profound spiritual or mysti- 
cal experiences in which the whole world is perceived 
to be one with God, Christ, or some other Divine 
entity. However, there is still one or more objects in 
that perception of reality (i.e. the universe and the 
Divine entity). 

Absolute unitary states form a fundamentally 
different type of primary epistemic state. In unitary 
reality, there is no perception of discrete, independent 
objects that can be related to each other so there 
cannot be any relationships (regular or irregular). As 
mentioned, there may be many other states, espe- 
cially spiritual ones, that have a significant degree of 
unitary experience even though the totality of every- 
thing is not considered to be completely unified. Uni- 
tary states other than absolute unity most likely repre- 
sent a number of spiritual or mystical states, but 
probably lie along the continuum of primary epis- 
temic states between those that involve the perception 
of multiple discrete objects and those in which there 
is the perception of a unity without discrete objects. 
The absolute unitary state referred to in this discus- 
sion represents a state described in many religious 
and philosophical perspectives. Thus, Nirvana, Abso- 
lute Reality, the Oneness of God, Absolute Unitary 
Being, and a number of other terms all refer to this 
complete and total unitary experience of the universe 
(d'Aquili and Newberg 1999). The exact physiology 

59 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



of such a state is also an interesting issue since a re- 
searcher can never know when such an experience is 
being perceived. However, research has suggested 
some possible correlates. Most likely, areas that sub- 
serve the sense of self and the sense of space and 
time are affected. It may be that activity inherently 
within these areas is substantially decreased, or per- 
haps neuronal activity going into or coming out of 
those areas is blocked (i.e. these areas are cut off from 
the rest of the brain's functions). 

It might be argued that the unitary reality state is 
associated with three possible emotional states which 
contain either positive, negative, or neutral affect, 
similarly to the states in which there is the experience 
of discrete objects. If unitary reality is associated with 
positive affect it is perceived as an undifferentiated 
oneness which is totally joyful and overwhelmingly 
good. The experience of unitary reality with neutral 
affect is very similar to the experience of unitary real- 
ity with positive affect such that the universe is di- 
rectly understood as being an undifferentiated one- 
ness. However, with neutral affect the oneness is un- 
derstood on a very impersonal level. Unitary reality is 
not viewed as good or bad or anything - it just is. 
Thus, the state of unitary reality with neutral affect 
would more likely be referred to as the void or infinite 
nothingness in religious literature. This is particularly 
the case in Buddhist philosophy. It is interesting to 
note that, to date, there are no clear references to an 
experience of a unitary reality when perceived with a 
negative affect. It may be that such a state simply is 
not possible. Perhaps it cannot come about because 
the experience of all things as an undifferentiated 
oneness is so powerfully positive and integrative, that 
it cannot be perceived in negative terms. It may be 
argued that such an experience of unitary reality with 
negative affect is even incompatible with life, the 
brain, or the mind. 

An important point about the unitary epistemic 
states is that it could be further argued that the uni- 
tary reality state should actually include all three pos- 
sible emotional states together, since even affect 
should be experienced as a unity. In other words, this 
state cannot even be considered to have different af- 
fective components. This might also be the case since 
the perceiving self is not separate from the rest of the 
universe in the unitary epistemic state, and thus, any 
emotion can theoretically only be felt after the person 
is no longer in the epistemic state. They can only re- 
flect on the emotional response they have as the result 
of being in the unitary epistemic state since there is 
no self to have the emotion during that state. It is not 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



clear what the experience of positive, negative, and 
neutral emotions all combined into one would actu- 
ally feel like. Arguably, it might be experienced as 
neutral since the positive and negative would cancel 
out. But since the positive and negative would theo- 
retically be included in the neutral, it still might be a 
different experience from a state which is simply neu- 
tral. It is also not clear how such a state might corre- 
late with neurological functions although it may be 
possible for structures that are associated with positive 
affect and those associated with negative affect to be 
activated at the same time. Descriptions of such uni- 
tary states do utilize a wide variety of emotions, 
sometimes together, ranging from fear and awe to joy 
and utter contentment. However, in the end, it might 
be argued that unitary reality should not be differen- 
tiated, even by affect. 

The most important aspect of the primary epis- 
temic state of unitary reality is that unlike other pri- 
mary states, when an individual 'comes out of it,' 
evidence suggests that the person does not perceive it 
or the memory of it as an illusion, hallucination, or 
delusion (Newberg and Waldman 2009). Once a per- 
son has been in the state of unitary reality, they un- 
derstand it to exist even though the person may not 
be in those states at some later time. Thus, the state of 
unitary reality appears to violate the rule of primary 
epistemic states, that they are real when in them and 
are perceived as not real when in another primary 
epistemic state. When reality is experienced as uni- 
tary, the person believes this state to be fundamentally 
real regardless of which other state they are in. In 
fact, the sense of reality is so strong during the expe- 
rience of unitary reality, that when a person comes 
out of this experience and enters into another pri- 
mary epistemic state, the new state is often perceived 
as a mere reflection or distortion of the unitary real- 
ity. Thus, unitary reality is perceived as real beyond 
all other primary states even when a person is in 
those other states. This property is unique to the ex- 
perience of unitary reality since no other primary 
epistemic state is perceived of as ultimate reality once 
one has moved from it to another primary state. 

Conclusions: 
Implications for Consciousness 

This chapter explored the nature of spiritual experi- 
ences from a neurohermeneutical and neurotheologi- 
cal perspective. The result is a multidisciplinary ap- 
proach that combines phenomenology, neuroscience, 
spirituality, anthropology, and theology. The result is 

60 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



an analysis of human experience that provides in- 
formation on how we perceive reality itself and make 
meaning about that reality through the processes of 
the brain. What is also intriguing about such an 
analysis is the place the consciousness has in all of 
this. On one hand, it might be argued that con- 
sciousness is an essential part of all experiences of 
reality and hence all of the primary epistemic states. 
This typically refers to a type of 'everyday' con- 
sciousness that human beings have. We all feel that 
we are conscious, have subjective awareness about the 
environment around us, and have an awareness of 
our internal state that responds to the external envi- 
ronment. This type of consciousness is often consid- 
ered from a more materialistic perspective in that it 
derives in large part from the functioning of the hu- 
man brain. As the brain processes information about 
the world, it provides a sense of awareness. Of course 
there are some fundamental philosophical and scien- 
tific problems with this notion of consciousness since 
it is not clear how a set of non-conscious biological 
processes can produce consciousness, let alone subjec- 
tive awareness if we define these two concepts sepa- 
rately. 

This is what scholars studying consciousness 
(Chalmers 1995) have come to call the 'hard prob- 
lem' — How does the brain function in such a way as 
to produce consciousness? Of course, part of the is- 
sue is that there is a presumed causal arrow from the 
brain to consciousness. This is consistent with the 
current paradigm of Western thought and science. 
However, there may be a different perspective to take. 
It may be that consciousness does not derive from 
biology, but rather, the other way around. Perhaps 
consciousness is the primary 'stuff' of the universe 
from which material reality is derived. This is cer- 
tainly consistent with Eastern traditions which hold 
consciousness to exist beyond simply the biology of 
the brain. What is also interesting in this regard is 
that the primary epistemic state of absolute unitary 
being is frequently experienced as a kind of universal 
consciousness. Such experiences contribute to the 
notion of a universal consciousness which is not pro- 
duced via the biology of the brain. The notion of 
how different philosophical concepts arise and are 
attributed to an explanation of reality may be directly 
related to the primary epistemic state in which the 
notion arises. For example, an individual who resides 
in the everyday reality epistemic state is more likely to 
find a materialistic cause of things. However, and 
individual who has experienced absolute unity may 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



be more likely to comprehend a spiritual or con- 
sciousness basis for reality. 

Neurohermeneutics and neurotheology are perspec- 
tives that would argue for a careful analysis of epis- 
temological claims and claims about the true nature 
of reality. Further, such a perspective would argue 
that an individual's primary epistemic state, including 
the brain states involved, are a crucial piece of in- 
formation that leads to the nature of any person's 
beliefs about reality. But neurohermeneutics and neu- 
rotheology also remind us that every belief, concept, 
or experience we hold about the world is processed in 
some way through the human brain. Therefore, we 
can never know if our ideas about reality, or our ex- 
perience of a primary epistemic state is more or less 
real than any other. This fundamental problem lies at 
the heart of epistemology spirituality, religion, and 
science. Neurohermeneutics and neurotheology, rec- 
ognize this problem and try to offer a multidisciplin- 
ary approach that might get us closer to resolving this 
problem than any singular approach. But the ulti- 
mate answers to these questions for now remain elu- 
sive. 

References 

Chalmers DJ (1995). 'Facing up to the problem of 
consciousness.' Journal of Consciousness Studies. 
2:200-219. 

d'Aquili EG (1978). 'The neurobiological bases of 
myth and concepts of deity.' ZyS on - 
13(4):257-275. 

d'Aquili EG (1982). 'Senses of reality in science and 
religion.' Zygon. 17:361-384. 

d'Aquili EG (1986). 'Myth, ritual and the archetypal 
hypothesis: Does the dance generate the word?' 
Zygon. 2 1(2): 141-160. 

d'Aquili EG and Newberg AB (1999). The Mystical 
Mind: Probing the Biology of Religious Experience. 
Minneapolis, MN: Fortress Press. 

dAquili EG (1985). 'Human ceremonial ritual and 
the modulation of aggression.' Zjgon. 
20(l):21-30. 

dAquili EG and Newberg AB (1993). 'Religious and 
mystical states: a neuropsychological substrate.' 
Zygon. 28: 177-200. 

61 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



d'Aquili EG and Newberg AB (1998). 'The neuropsy- 
chological basis of religion: Or why God won't 
go away' fygon. 33(2): 187-201. 

Herzog H, Lele VR, Kuwert T, et al. (1990-1991). 
'Changed pattern of regional glucose metabo- 
lism during yoga meditative relaxation.' Neuropsy- 
chobiology. 23: 182-187. 

Joseph R (1996). Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry and 
Behavioral Neurology. Baltimore: Williams & Wilk- 
ins. 

Lazar SW, Bush G, Gollub RL, et al. (2000). 'Func- 
tional brain mapping of the relaxation response 
and meditation.' Neuroreport. 11: 1581-1585. 

Newberg A, Alavi A, Baime M, Mozley PD and 
d'Aquili E (2001). 'The measurement of regional 
cerebral blood flow during the complex cogni- 
tive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT 
study' Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. 106: 113- 
122. 




Biography 

Andrew Newberg, M.D. Is 
Director of Research at the 
Myrna Brind Center for Inte- 
grative Medicine at Thomas 
Jefferson University Hospital 
and Medical College, an 
Adjunct Professor of Relig- 
ious Studies and an Associ- 
ate Professor of Radiology 
at the University of Pennsyl- 
vania School of Medicine. 
He has been a prominent 
researcher in the field of 
nuclear medical brain imag- 
ing. In particular, his research has focused on the devel- 
opment of neurotransmitter tracers for the evaluation of 
religiosity as well as neurological and psychiatric disorders 
including clinical depression, head injury, Alzheimer's dis- 
ease, and Parkinson's disease. Because of his work in the 
intersection between religion and the brain, he has become 
an adjunct assistant professor in the Department of Relig- 
ious Studies at the University of Pennsylvania. From 2005 
to 201 0 he was also the Director of the Center for Spiritual- 
ity and the Mind. 



New Publication of Interest: 
Kuma Art & Shamanism - Paolo Fortis 



Newberg AB (2010). Principles of Neurotheology . Surrey 
UK: Ashgate Publishing Ltd. 

Newberg AB and Waldman MR (2009). How God 
Changes Tour Brain. New York: Ballantine. 

Penfield W and Perot P (1963). 'The brain's record of 
auditory and visual experience.' Brain. 86: 595- 
695. 

Smart N (1969). The Religious Experience of Mankind. 
London: Macmillan. 

Smart N (1978). 'Understanding Religious Experi- 
ence.' In: Katz S (ed), Mysticism and Philosophical 
Analysis. New York: Oxford University Press. 

Stace WT (1961). Mysticism and Philosophy. London: 
Macmillan. 

Valenstein ES (1973). Brain control: A critical examination 
of brain stimulation and psychosurgery. New York: 
John Wiley & Sons. 



UNA 
Art and Shamanist 




http://utpress.utexas.edu/index.php/books/forkun 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



62 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Announcing a New Masters Programme in Canterbury, UK 

MA in Myth, Cosmology and the Sacred 



To begin January 2014, 1 year full-time or 
2 years part-time 

Tutors: Wilma Fraser (director), Geoffrey 
Cornelius, Angela Voss, Marguerite 
Rigoglioso (guest lecturer). 

This interdisciplinary Masters programme 
draws on studies in psychology, anthro- 
pology, theology, esoteric philosophy, a 
range of wisdom traditions and the arts. It 
offers a discerning investigation into 
seemingly non-rational modes of know- 
ing, exploring the cosmological sense of 
the sacred, the widespread practices of 
symbol-interpretation and divination, and 
the cultural role of the creative imagina- 
tion. The programme will appeal to all 
those seeking to enrich their lives 
through the study of the history, philoso- 
phy and rituals of Western sacred and 
esoteric traditions, and will be of particu- 
lar interest to teachers, practitioners and 
therapists in the fields of contemporary 
spirituality and well-being who would like to engage more deeply with the foundations of their 
work. Students will be required to submit four essays, a creative portfolio and review, extracts 
from an ongoing reflective Learning Journal and a dissertation. The MA is taught at alternate 
weekends Jan-June, with additional Wednesday mornings for full time students. The second 
half of the year consists of supervised research with a presentation weekend in September. 
Students will be required to submit four essays, a creative portfolio and review, extracts from 
an ongoing reflective Learning Journal and a dissertation. 



For the student handbook and all admin information (including fees) contact Michelle Childs: 
post.compulsory.education@canterbury.ac.uk, 01227 863458. 



For information regarding course content, contact Angela Voss: 
angela.voss@canterbury.ac.uk 



http://www.canterbury.ac.uk/courses/prospectus/postgraduate/courses/myth-cosmology-sacred.asp 




Canterbury 

Department of Post Compulsory Education and Training Christ Church 

i University 



<2>i 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



63 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Ultra-Terrestrials and the UFO Phenomenon: 
A Response to Steven Mizrach 

Jason Colavito 




In the previous issue of this journal (Vol. 4, No. 2), 
Steven Mizrach discussed "The Para-Anthropology 
of UFO Abductions: The Case for UTH," or the 
"ultra-terrestrial hypothesis." This is an idea put for- 
ward by Jacques Vallee, the famed ufologist, that the 
phenomena associated with UFOs and alien abduc- 
tion could be explained by the actions of intelligent 
beings from another dimension manipulating human 
consciousness. In this, Vallee was preceded by science 
fiction writers like H. P Lovecraft who used the same 
conceit in stories like "The Dreams in the Witch 
House" (1932) and "The Dunwich Horror" (1928), 
and earlier by the Victorian-era Spiritualists and 
Theosophists who argued that beings from other di- 
mensions played a direct role in human affairs. For 
the Spiritualists, these were four-dimensional beings 
whose actions had consequences in three dimensions, 
while the Theosophists posited myriad parallel di- 
mensions centred on other solar system bodies and 
populated by beings that moved into and out of this 
dimension to induce evolutionary change on earth. 

Mizrach evaluated three competing explanations 
for the UFO phenomenon: (a) the traditional view 
that UFOs are nuts-and-bolts spaceships (the extra- 
terrestrial hypothesis, or ETH), the more recent view 
that UFOs can be explained as modern folklore (the 
psychocultural hypothesis, or PCH), and the UTH. 
Mizrach, however, said that he was unsatisfied with 
the folklore explanation because for him it failed to 
explain the power of the UFO phenomenon and its 
effect on the lives of those who encounter alien be- 
ings. 

Thus, Mizrach concludes that the UTH is the 
best remaining explanation for the UFO phenome- 
non, following Sherlock Holmes' fictional dictum that 
eliminating the impossible leaves by default the truth: 

The one thing I am sure of, however, is that 
there is an intelligence behind the phenome- 
non [. . .] The ETH fails, but I also find the 
'pure' form of the PCH insufficient, so I turn 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



to Vallee's UTH (sometimes also known as the 
EDI, or extra-dimensional intelligence theory), 
as the best model, for now. 

Mizrach also asserts that modern science has dis- 
counted the possibility of the UFO phenomenon 
providing important or even interesting scientific 
data, and he claims that the United States govern- 
ment exerted influence in discounting the value of 
UFO research. 

In this article, I would like to challenge both of 
these views, beginning with Mizrach's argument that 
science sees no value in UFO research. 

The Condon Report 

Mizrach's argument in support of the UTH as the 
best model for the UFO phenomenon is predicated 
upon the supposition that the University of Colorado 
UFO Project's Air Force-funded 1969 Scientific 
Study of Unidentified Flying Objects, better known 
as the Condon Report, acted as a mission statement for 
scientists, and that it discounted the value of UFO 
research. The Report wrote that those scientists who 
studied UFOs concluded "that UFO phenomena do 
not offer a fruitful field in which to look for major 
scientific discoveries" (1969:2). 

This is Mizrach's warrant for suggesting that 
three "alternatives" to mainstream science (the ETH, 
PCH, and UTH) are therefore plausible avenues for 
research given the silence from science, which has 
declared UFOs incompatible with physics: 

I mean, even if the essential model is correct, 
science could still learn something from study- 
ing UFO reports. Perhaps we could learn more 
about human misperception of stars and plan- 
ets, the inability for people to correctly esti- 
mate the size or distance of aerial objects, or 
even the mechanisms behind the confabulation 
of false stories. Yet, that is the mantra of the 



64 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



1969 report, that nothing of scientific value 
can be gained from studying UFO reports, and 
therefore the Air Force and other branches of 
government have no need to investigate them. 

However, Mizrach has misrepresented the Condon 
Report, for, while the report opens with the conclusion 
that UFOs have nothing to teach about extraterres- 
trial beings, just a few paragraphs later, the report 
directly contradicts Mizrach's version of its contents: 

As the reader of this report will readily judge, 
we have focussed attention almost entirely on 
the physical sciences. This was in part a matter 
of determining priorities and in part because 
we found rather less than some persons may 
have expected in the way of psychiatric prob- 
lems related to belief in the reality of UFOs as 
craft from remote galactic or intergalactic civi- 
lizations. We believe that the rigorous study of 
the beliefs — unsupported by valid evi- 
dence — held by individuals and even by some 
groups might prove of scientific value to the 
social and behavioral sciences. There is no im- 
plication here that individual or group psycho- 
pathology is a principal area of study. Reports 
of UFOs offer interesting challenges to the 
student of cognitive processes as they are af- 
fected by individual and social variables. By 
this connection, we conclude that a content- 
analysis of press and television coverage of 
UFO reports might yield data of value both to 
the social scientist and the communications 
specialist. The lack of such a study in the pre- 
sent report is due to a judgment on our part 
that other areas of investigation were of much 
higher priority. We do not suggest, however, 
that the UFO phenomenon is, by its nature, 
more amenable to study in these disciplines 
than in the physical sciences. On the contrary, 
we conclude that the same specificity in pro- 
posed research in these areas is as desirable as 
it is in the physical sciences. 

Given that the report which Mizrach says determined 
the scientific view of UFOs asserts the value of social 
and behavior sciences for understanding the UFO 
phenomenon, it is therefore not permissible to classify 
the PCH as an "alternative" viewpoint tacitly coequal 
with the ETH and UTC. Rather, the PCH should be 
seen as the default (social) scientific explanation for 
the UFO phenomenon insofar as such a phenome- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



non exists (a point to which I will return). Thus, the 
work of Thomas E. Bullard (1989) in locating alien 
abduction claims in the context of traditional super- 
natural abduction narratives, and the work of Susan 
A. Clancy (2005) probing the psychological origins of 
abduction narratives are not "alternatives" to consen- 
sus science but rather are operating entirely within 
the mainstream. If the "hard" sciences conclude that 
UFOs are impossible under the laws of physics as 
currently known, it does not follow that no scholar- 
ship emerges to explain why such claims persist. 

It is therefore not nearly as surprising as Mizrach 
claims that U.S. government agencies continued to 
record UFO sightings after the Condon Report. They 
did not "ignore the findings of the report" but rather 
followed its broader view. Or, more likely, they moni- 
tored such reports for what they could tell the gov- 
ernment about Soviet aircraft and public perception 
of secret American craft. The American government, 
after all, is documented to have planted a fake UFO 
report in the Soviet press in connection with U.S. ef- 
forts to monitor Soviet activity in a strategically valu- 
able Norwegian island in the Arctic (U.S. Dept. of 
State 1968; see also Colavito 2013). In other words, 
UFOs held interest for the government, just not in 
the context of extraterrestrial (or ultra-terrestrial) visi- 
tors. 

The Ultra-Terrestrial Hypothesis 

Strictly speaking, scientific research into PCH cannot 
preclude UTH. Even if it can be shown that alien 
abduction reports emerge primarily in the context of 
altered states of consciousness (ASC), typically during 
the transition between waking and sleep, this cannot 
categorically exclude the arrival of trans-dimensional 
intelligences at precisely that moment, nor can it pre- 
clude the idea that our dreams are excursions to the 
otherworldly beings' homelands. However, by the 
same token, science cannot categorically exclude 
phlogiston, pink elephants, or the Greek gods, no 
matter how vanishingly remote the possibility of their 
existence. 

The better question is: With what warrant do we 
propose the existence of ultra-terrestrial beings? For 
Mizrach, summarizing Vallee, and to a lesser extent 
Carl Jung, the answer is that PCH explanations are 
"unsatisfying" for two reasons: (a) alien encounters 
are too powerful and emotionally moving to be ex- 
plained as the product of the human mind, and (b) 
UFO reports have physical correlates in the material 
world that cannot be explained by appeals to mental 

65 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



events. I would submit that both of these claims are 
false, though for different reasons. 

The question of the reality of mental events is a 
qualitative judgment. Mizrach recognizes that abduc- 
tion reports are nearly identical to shamanic encoun- 
ters with the gods in ASC, yet he "struggled with this 
as an explanation of sufficient power" to account for 
abductees' changes in personality and ideology, as 
well as the appearance of the aliens to multiple wit- 
nesses. Multiple witnesses, however, he illustrates with 
the Travis Walton case, which skeptics have debunked 
quite clearly as a combination of financial despera- 
tion on the part of a group of failing loggers (he and 
his fellow witnesses would owe a significant financial 
penalty unless an "act of God" intervened) and a 
convenient viewing of NBC's TV-movie about the 
Betty and Barney Hill abduction just two weeks ear- 
lier. A second case Mizrach cites, the Allagash "ab- 
duction" of four men, is similarly suspect due to the 
use of dubious regression hypnosis to generate the 
abduction report. Surely a hypothesis as radical as 
UTH requires evidence with firmer foundations. 

That leaves the question of the intensity of the 
experience. Surely this is an exquisitely subjective 
question, for there is no reliable way to judge whether 
a mental phenomenon is sufficiently intense to objec- 
tively warrant a change in behavior. Schizophrenics, 
of course, experience intense auditory hallucinations 
sufficient for them to justify changes in their behavior 
in response to these self-generated stimuli; however, 
very few even among fringe researchers consider this 
evidence of schizophrenic brains tuning in to alien 
wavelengths. In 2002, David Lewis-Williams pro- 
posed that modern human consciousness emerged 
during the Upper Paleolithic in connection with 
ASC, that the neurology of the human brain gener- 
ated stock images in responses to ASC, and that cul- 
ture defines how the individual experiences those 
stock images — as vortexes, tunnels, gods, monsters, 
aliens, etc. 

For Lewis-Williams, the intensity of these ASC 
experiences is what drove the emergence of higher 
order consciousness, and since each piece of the ASC 
experience can be induced experimentally by stimula- 
tion of the brain, the phenomenon as a whole is 
therefore reducible to the neurological function of 
the brain. Lewis-Williams and David Pearce (2005) 
later expanded this argument to show how the same 
processes are reflected in Neolithic culture, centered 
on a belief in the ability of the individual to travel 
through a vortex to meet with the ancestral spirits or 
the gods. The authors then connected this to modern 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



cultures' experiences and experimental laboratory 
results. Thus, from the earliest human cultures, all of 
the elements of the classic alien abduction were in 
place, and they could be demonstrated to draw from 
neurological — that is material, earthbound — sources. 
As I noted above, this cannot be strictly proved since 
the trans-dimensional aliens might well use ASC as a 
gateway from their dimension, as advocated by Gra- 
ham Hancock (2007) in adapting Lewis-Williams 
work, but like phlogiston to the flame, the trans- 
dimensional beings become somewhat redundant for 
explaining alien abductions in the wake of neurologi- 
cal evidence. 

Is There a Singular UFO Phenomenon? 

A complicating factor that Lewis-Williams's work 
creates for the UTH is the fact that shamanic ASC 
and historical "abduction" experiences, cited by 
Vallee and other UTH speculators, do not conform 
to the full narrative of the modern UFO phenome- 
non, as developed after the Betty and Barney Hill 
abduction claim (Fuller 1 966) and J. Allen Hynek's 
(1972) classification of three types of UFO encoun- 
ters, culminating with contact. Prior to this, strange 
lights in the sky were not generally found in conjunc- 
tion with other staples of the narrative, such as ab- 
duction, sexual experimentation, and cattle mutila- 
tion, a fact even the credulous Vallee (2009) himself 
seemed to concede in cataloguing the "best" evidence 
for prehistoric UFOs and finding no unambiguous 
evidence for a complete UFO narrative prior to the 
modern era, only fragments that paralleled portions 
of the modern narrative. This might mean that the 
trans-dimensional beings first emerged into our di- 
mension only in 1947, 1961, or some other date, but 
this would not explain those partial parallels. 

I have previously traced the Hill abduction to 
alien encounter and medical experimentation motifs 
derived from three consecutive episodes of The Outer 
Limits (1964), airing over the three weeks prior to 
Barney Hill's first hypnosis session, including the 
slanted-eyed aliens and their distinctive clothing, the 
invasive probing, the backwoods setting, and even an 
interracial narrative paralleling the Hills' own ro- 
mance (Colavito 2012). It is noteworthy that the Hills 
originally only reported to Project Bluebook seeing a 
flying saucer until they were placed in an altered state 
of consciousness three years later and began recalling 
abduction imagery exactly paralleling Outer Limits 
episodes in both plot and aesthetics from the weeks 
before hypnosis. This origin point for the classic ab- 

66 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



duction narrative strongly favors the PCH over the 
UTH if this order of events is correct. Given that 
high profile abduction cases that followed, including 
the Travis Walton incident, can be shown to repro- 
duce ideas and imagery appearing originally with the 
Hill case, this again favors PCH over UTH. 

Since Mizrach cited Sherlock Holmes about ac- 
ceptance of the improbable, it is only fair to mention 
Occam's Razor in defense of the idea that the hy- 
pothesis with fewer assumptions is more likely to be 
correct; in this case, the proposal of an unseen and 
unattested alternative dimension of reality, populated 
by multiple beings of near-supernatural intelligence, 
who are capable of interacting with this dimension in 
fixed ways across time and space is vastly more com- 
plicated than the alternatives. The only serious sup- 
port for this claim is the contention that the UFO 
phenomenon encompasses physical phenome- 
na — such as UFOs that can be tracked on ra- 
dar — that preclude a purely mental explanation. In- 
deed, this is Mizrach's primary objection to PCH. 
This leads to my final question: Is the UFO phe- 
nomenon singular? 

The modern UFO phenomenon is composed 
(roughly) of four parts: UFO sightings, crop circles, 
cattle mutilation, and alien abduction. Ufologists dis- 
agree on whether crop circles and cattle mutilation 
should be considered part of the phenomenon, and 
alternative explanations exist even among believers. 
Cattle mutilation, for example, was traditionally as- 
cribed down to the Twentieth Century to the evil 
power of the goatsucker (nightjar), a (real) bird whose 
mythology was reapplied to the Chupacabra, whose 
name (literally: "goat sucker") belies its origins (see 
my chapter on the Chupacabra in Colavito 2013), 
and provides an equally incredible explanation for 
something science recognizes as natural decay. Simi- 
larly, prior to the modern UFO myth, lights in the sky 
were treated as a distinct class of "prodigy" from noc- 
turnal visitation by strange visitors such as incubi and 
succubae, whom Vallee and Bullard both see as 
analogous to UFO denizens. These visitations, how- 
ever, were not associated with spaceships or intense 
light, just kinky sex. Additionally, the first reported 
alien encounters — those from before the Hills like 
George Adamski's — were wildly diverse, including 
civilized diplomatic meetings with Nordic -looking 
aliens from Venus, like those of Golden Age science 
fiction, as filtered through Theosophy. It is only after 
the 1960s that these threads come together in the 
modern UFO myth. 



Because we find the various elements of the UFO 
myth in isolation throughout history, the logical con- 
clusion is that the four facets of the myth were origi- 
nally separate and brought together because of the 
UFO myth and the UFO phenomenon is not the 
cause of the four facets. In this an instructive parallel 
can be found in the ancient Greek myth of giants 
who (a) built the massive Mycenaean ruins, (b) left 
behind their gigantic bones, and (c) performed magic 
from their underground tombs and rose to communi- 
cate with those who sacrificed to them. The myth 
emerged from mistakes (about the origin of ruins and 
about the giant bones, really those of extinct Pleisto- 
cene mammals — see Mayor [2000]), and religious 
ideology, but it seemed supported by facts which were 
forever after linked to the myth. In the same way, the 
modern UFO myth is leading researchers down the 
path of proposing elaborate explanations for a phe- 
nomenon that cannot yet be proved to require a sin- 
gular explanation. 

If treating sightings, abductions, mutilations, and 
crop circles as distinct events yields productive expla- 
nations for each (as skeptics contend), then the UFO 
phenomenon as a whole may be considered as a 
modern myth and the UTH can be discarded as re- 
dundant, though as with phlogiston and unicorns, it 
cannot be conclusively proven wrong, only unneces- 
sary. This then frees the researcher to examine multi- 
ple causes for various phenomena, from ASC for 
most abduction cases to a wide range of events that 
yield lights in the sky. By discarding the strictures of 
forcing all of the factors of contemporary UFO my- 
thology to conform to a single hypothesis, the truth 
may in fact emerge more fully and brilliantly than 
ufologists suspect. 

References 

Bullard, T. E. (1989). 'UFO abduction reports: The 
supernatural kidnap narrative returns in techno- 
logical guise.' journal of American Folklore, 
102(404): 147-170. 

Clancy, S. A. (2005). Abducted: How People Come to Be- 
lieve They Were Kidnapped by Aliens. Cambridge: 
Harvard. 

Colavito, J. (2013). Faking History: Essays on Aliens, At- 
lantis, Monsters, and More. Albany: 
JasonColavito.com Books. 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



67 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Colavito, J. (2012). Alien abduction at the outer lim- 
its.' [Online] JasonColavito.com. 
http:/ /www.jasoncolavito.com/ alien-abduction- 
at-the-outer-limits.html [Accessed June 6, 2013]. 

Condon, E. U. (1968). Scientific Study of Unidentified 
Flying Objects. University of Colorado. 

Fuller, J. G. (1966). The Interrupted Journey. New York: 
Dial Press. 

Hancock, G. (2007). Supernatural: Meetings with the An- 
cient Teachers of Mankind. New York: Disinforma- 
tion. 

Hynek, J. A. (1972). The UFO Experience: A Scientific 
Inquiry. Chicago: Regnery. 

Lewis- Williams, D. (2002). The Mind in the Cave. Lon- 
don: Thames & Hudson. 

Lewis-Williams, D. and Pearce, D. (2005). Inside the 
Neolithic Mind. London: Thames & Hudson. 

Mayor, A. (2000). The First Fossil Hunters: Paleontology in 
Greek and Roman Times. Princeton: Princeton Uni- 
versity Press. 

Mizrach, S. (2013). 'The para-anthropology of UFO 
abductions: The case for the UTH.' Paranthropol- 
ogj, 4(2): 4-18. 

Outer Limits, The. (1964). ABC-TV [Television 
Broadcasts]: 'The Invisibles,' February 3; The 
Bellero Shield, February 10; The Children of 
Spider County, February 17. 

U.S. Department of State (1968). 'Flying Saucers Are 
a Myth' [Online]. Airgram No. A-1221, March 22. 
Available from: National Security Agency FOIA 
Reading Room [Accessed June 6, 2013]. 

Vallee, J. and Aubeck, C. (2009). Wonders in the Sky. 
New York: Penguin. 




Biography 

Jason Colavito is an author 
and editor based in Albany, 
NY, whose books include The 
Cult of Alien Gods: H.P. Love- 
craft and Extraterrestrial Pop 
Culture (Prometheus Books, 
2005); Knowing Fear (McFar- 
land, 2008); and more. Co- 
lavito is internationally recog- 
nized by scholars, literary 
theorists, and scientists for his 
pioneering work exploring the 
connections between science, 
pseudoscience, and specula- 
tive fiction. His investigations, 

which have appeared on the History Channel and were 
cited in publications like The Atlantic and The Huffington 
Post, examine the way human beings create and employ 
the supernatural to alter and understand our reality and our 
world. 



New Publication of Interest: 
'Mystic Chemist: The Life of Albert 
Hofmann and His Discovery of LSD' 

More than just a biography, this book is a 
social and cultural history of LSD from its 
discovery in 1 938 to the present day. 
Highly recommended. 



MYSTIC 




The Life of 
Albert Hofmann 
and His 
Discovery of 
LSD 




Dieter Hagenbach and Lucius Werthmiiller 
Foreword by Stanislav Grof 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



68 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Musings on Good, Evil and the Conquest of Mexico 
An Interview With Graham Hancock 

William Rowlandson 




That s what demons do - multiply human misery. 

In frosty February I visited Graham Hancock in his 
house in Bath to interview him for Paranthropology. I 
had met Graham and his wife Santha at the confer- 
ence on psychedelics, Breaking Convention, in 201 1, and 
towards the end of a long conversation over coffee 
Graham mentioned that his new work - a series of 
novels - would be about Cortes and Moctezuma and 
the daimonic/ demonic forces urging them towards 
their explosive encounter. Intrigued, I made contact 
with Graham some months later to suggest an inter- 
view and to seek a review copy of the novel. 

War God is a gripping and frank depiction of the 
very real horror that accompanied this period, and 
the reader is not shielded from visions of this harsh 
reality There is a confident balance between histori- 
cal realism: conversations, sights, smells, sounds, gore 
- and inspired imagination: Moctezuma's mushroom- 
fuelled relationship with the hummingbird god 
Huitzilopochtli, and Cortes' dream-dialogue with St 
Peter. Importantly, Graham presents with candour 
and a genuinely open enquiry the very plausible pos- 
sibilities of this communication between humans and 
deities. Making no categorical statements about the 
ontological nature of these entities, i.e. whether em- 
pirically distinct beings or complexes of the uncon- 
scious, the novel demonstrates that the sheer scale of 
slaughter and destruction was inseparable from the 
religious fervour of all parties. Something drove these 
historical figures beyond simple lust for power. This 
mystery is explored in War God. 

Of all the many questions that I was keen to pose, 
it became clear that the depiction of violence and the 
sense of relentless brutality were the matters that 
most preoccupied me. Graham had told me that his 
first work of fiction, Entangled, had been inspired by 
some visions during ayahuasca sessions, and that he 
had experienced similar visions of the historical pe- 
riod of Cortes and the Aztecs. Neither Entangled nor 
War God shirk from presenting horrific cruelty perpe- 
trated by humans as well as by entities of the spirit 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



world who exert their malevolent influence over hu- 
mankind. In this respect, acts of love and compas- 
sion, in opposition to acts of hate and horror, are 
woven into the fabric of reality as part of a cosmic 
conflict between good and evil. There is thus a meta- 
physical, perhaps theological, aspect to these novels, 
and I was intrigued to seek correlatives between Gra- 
ham's fiction and his other works concerning ancient 
architecture, lost civilisations, occult gnostic thought, 
shamanism, psychedelics, and political power. Over 
the course of the interview, it became clear that what 
to some readers may appear as a radical departure 
from his investigative, non-fiction work was in essence 
an integral component of a larger vision presented 
throughout his works of this ancient confrontation of 
forces. It is also noteworthy that whilst the interview 
lasted over two hours and covered matters as diverse 
as ayahuasca-inspired artwork, the Jesuits, Aztec sac- 
rifices, the War on Terror, drone strikes, cannabis and 
megaliths, Graham chose to post on YouTube a 20- 
minute segment of the interview in which he dis- 
cusses precisely this matter of dualism, which he enti- 
tled 'Graham Hancock on Good and Evil.' This topic 
was clearly at the heart of the interview. 

To begin the interview, I asked what role ayahua- 
sca had played in providing the visions for the novels. 
A great deal, was his response, and in particular, he 
explained that the narrative of Entangled appeared 
almost complete in a vision with a core sense that he 
had been mistaken about the Neanderthals, and that, 
importantly, he felt emboldened to present them in 
the true nature that he had sensed in the vision: 

Graham: / can't paint. In my case I have a gift of writing. 
Ayahuasca did directly influence me, partly because I asked it 
to. I published Entangled in 2007. I went to Brazil with the 
intention of seeking a novel — it's useful to have some intention 

— let the vine show me what it wants to show me — or — I said 
that I would like to be given a vision. I felt tired of non-fiction 
—footnotes, etc. — rigorous defence of arguments against attack 

- I wanted freedom - I was also getting older and wanted 
something new. Ayahuasca gave me an instant answer — the 

69 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



story across time — good against evil. Two entangled women — 
one in the past one in the present — are brought together across 
time by this angelic being an entity whom I associate with 
Mother Ayahuasca. She could be real, she could be in our mind 
— who cares — most people experiencing ayahuasca see her. The 
Blue Angel is the benign supernatural force — who brings the 
two women together to do battle against the demon who wishes 
us to take a dark and evil path. And role of women is to resist. 
They have to resist as they understand that some force in the 
universe wants us not to recognise that we have a divine spark. I 
was shown scenes, battles and episodes, and I was told 'Han- 
cock you were wrong about the Neanderthals'. 

I first drank ayahuasca when researching for the book Su- 
pernatural as I had wanted to experience first-hand these 
shamanic altered states of consciousness rather than just reading 
about them. In that book I had misrepresented the Neander- 
thals. I was persuaded by what was then the mainstream aca- 
demic line — that they were not symbolic creatures — that yes 
they had big brains — bigger than ours —yes they had been in 
command of Ice-Age Europe for 200,000 years — but they 
were stupid, brutish, no symbolic or spiritual life — their burials 
were unimportant — their culture was accidental, etc. And I had 
thought fair enough' and I depicted them that way. But what I 
was shown in my ayahuasca visions was that it was not so — 
they were creative, spiritual creatures. Our ancestors would not 
have survived had they not learnt from the Neanderthals. It was 
a revelation to me that I got it all wrong that I had done them 
disservice. Entangled was my opportunity to put it right — to 
show that Neanderthals were good and decent people — they 
were pure love and goodness who communicated telepathically — 
and that although they left little cave painting they painted their 
bodies. Furthermore, they taught our ancestors how to paint. 
Researchers have shown that origin of modern human behav- 
iour may well have been brought about by assimilation. Terence 
McKenna even thought this in Food for the Gods. It came to 
me in inspiration that the Neanderthals introduced our ancestors 
to sacred visionary medicines, from which they understood about 
painting. There may even have been interbreeding and current 
research is investigating that. The Neanderthals taught our 
ancestors all this, and I feel I've done them justice in the novel. 
I've portrayed them as my vision — as goodness and love — and 
now academic research is proving this theory right, that they 
may have even interbred. All this I received in the vision, and it 
makes me quite emotional. I got many insights in the visions — 
at the heart of it is the dualistic view of the universe — battle of 
good and evil and the importance of humans to choose. A catch 
line — evil may not always be defeated, but it will always be 
resisted. 

William: Human life has always been about territo- 
rial conflict, batdes, skirmishes. Why the need to pre- 
sent it as a choice between good and evil rather than 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



many cultural, social even biological polarities that do 
not imply such value? 

Graham: 50 to 40,000 years ago the first anatomically 
modern humans arrived in Western Europe. I see no evidence 
for violent, negative, aggressive behaviour. The fundamental 
question in Entangled is what actually happened to the Ne- 
anderthals? This is a mystery of prehistory. One theory is that 
our ancestors, having co-existed with them for 20,000 years, 
ended up wiping them out. Perhaps something in them changed 
- some influence — that caused them suddenly to turn on the 
Neanderthals. That possibility is left open in Entangled - but 
it is presented. This is the crux of Entangled. Over 24,000 
years the two girls become entangled, and Ria feels that it is her 
duty to prevent our ancestors wiping them out, in order to pay 
off some karmic debt for having done so. The demonic form 
gains his psychic charge by encouraging the humans to wipe out 
the Neanderthals. What emerges in the sequence of novels is 
that annihilation by anatomically modern human beings is 
stopped, that they die out for other reasons, and that we don't 
incur this karmic debt. Something changes in humans from the 
end of the upper Palaeolithic into the Neolithic — we become 
violent. And that's where I entertain the notion of dark entities 
at the spiritual level, and positive energies of resistance. And the 
human dilemma is always to choose. 

A friend of mine calls this world of ours 'a university of 
duality. ' I think this is interesting. A lot of people reject the idea 
of duality and would like everything to be one — that it is all 
beautiful and good — and that all duality is projection. But I'm 
not sure how much we would have to learn in a world like that. 
I think that duality is a very useful teaching tool, and that 
without it is difficult to make choices. In this particular experi- 
ence duality has very important lessons to teach us — and if it 
were taken away what choices would we have to make? 

William: But is there not a danger that a choice 
might be made in the assumption that it was good, 
and that history shows it not to be so? Does duality 
not go against mystical traditions of unity - that the 
good and the bad are always together - a harmony of 
polarities? 

Graham: Interesting points. In ancient Egypt the duality 
was Horus versus Set. Horus and Set in the same head. I don't 
disagree that there isn't an overarching oneness, but I still feel 
that if you remove the duality you remove a useful teaching tool. 

Science tells us that it is all but an accident, a random proc- 
esses. There is no such thing as the spirit. When we die we die. 
End of story. There is no transcendent purpose. Science may be 
right. But it may be wrong — and another possibility must be 
considered. Consciousness may be a part of the universe rather 
than an epiphenomenon of brain activity. Our incarnation may 

70 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



be an opportunity to learn and to choose. Perhaps, as many 
spiritual traditions show, we have many opportunities; and 
consequently we have the choice - and yes, one culture's good 
may be another culture's bad. Few people would dispute, for 
example, that there are cases when the killing of a person may 
be justified. Does the action that I'm about to take add or reduce 
misery? If we ask ourselves those questions, what is good and 
evil become clearer to define. In the case of Hitler, had someone 
killed him in 1937, they would have prevented the transgression 
of the sovereignty of millions of people. 1 Very fine distinctions 
are to be made. But if we examine our hearts, we have some 
kind of compass on this - and that we do know the good and 
the bad — and that it is not entirely culturally constrained. 

William: In your novels, investing so much narrative 
space to the darkness, are you in some way exorcis- 
ing? Is your choice here to focus on the darkness a 
means to open it up? Is there some purpose? 

Graham: Yes, in the novels I do dwell to some extent on 
negative human behaviour. As a human and a writer it is diffi- 
cult, it causes me pain, but I feel it is essential. Why? Because 
as a species we have a capacity for denial. When something is 
bad we look away ~ and by looking away we allow the evil to 
flourish. We need to focus on it. Many people who don't like 
Entangled don't like it became of the violence. I've received 
many letters. They say that it is all goodness and light and we 
must focus on it. I respond that nothing in the novel is made up 
by me. All the darkness, such as the castration of one's enemies 
and the wearing of the genitals on one's head; all of this has 
happened. There are other forms of human sacrifice, some of 
which are still being practiced by humans today. If we turn 
away and concentrate only on goodness, we allow such matters 
to continue. So what I'm doing with this writing is to focus 
precisely on these matters. Ib put it right up front. Here it is. 
This happens. Deal with it. If we're going to move on - and at 
the moment the world is in a dark place — horrendous things are 
happening — we're not going to solve the problems by pretending 
they don't exist. And in order to confront them we have to ex- 
press them. 

And my writing expresses this — not with a sense of revel- 
ling in the horror, but to show the reader what these things are, 
and hopefully raise the opportunity to address them in the future. 
I also show goodness. I show choice. Cortes could have made 
different choices. Moctezuma could have made different choices 
— and I'm intrigued to think what could have happened to the 
world had Cortes not made the decisions he made. It seems im- 
plausible that Cortes should have achieved what he did with so 
few men. This gigantic and truly violent state of the Mexica 



(Aztecs) was immensely violent, practising human sacrifice. 
How could the bunch of freebooters overthrow this? I try to 
show how they managed it. Had Moctezuma acted with com- 
passion and diplomacy towards his fellow peoples; had he 
shown more tolerance and not acted as he did then they would 
not have allied themselves to Cortes. All these folk detested the 
Aztecs, and so it is very reasonable that these folk joined Cortes. 
Had the Aztecs made that choice, then Cortes would have been 
presented with a united font on the shores of eastern Mexico. 
Consequently the entire venture of European colonialism would 
have had to have been rethought. Had they been greeted by a 
powerful and united force in Mexico, I feel that it would have 
affected the entire way the Europe operated elsewhere. It's obvi- 
ous with the conquest of the Inca. It is also obvious more 
widely around the world that European powers were persuaded 
that they could easily destroy these so-called primitive societies. 

William: But in War God there seems little hope of 
any redemption, of any light that will resist this in- 
credible force of darkness. Can we assume this light 
will come? Will it come through Tozi and Malinal, or 
through a spiritual force like the Blue Angel in Entan- 
gled? " 

Graham: Yes - absolutely — there will be redemption. This 
is very much a part of books 2 and 3. Goodness is through 
Tozi, Malinal and Pepillo. What I'm trying to show is that 
even in the worst circumstances — in the midst of overwhelming 
wickedness — it is possible for goodness and love to thrive and 
prevail. World War Two shows this to be the case: in the midst 
of the worst atrocities there were examples of goodness. I don't 
want to turn away from the darkness of the period. Even if 
goodness didn't win, it did speak out for what we can achieve — 
and yes —perhaps through supernatural intervention. 

William: You suggest in Heaven's Mirror and Finger- 
prints of the Gods that the Mexica inherited from previ- 
ous cultures a religious practice of sacrifice that had 
been symbolic, and that, unable to relate to this sym- 
bolic perspective, they twisted and distorted the prac- 
tice into the very literal and real practice of human 
sacrifice. Have you developed this line of enquiry in 
your research and writing of War God and its sequels? 

Graham: Yes — my research over the years leads me to believe 
the appearance over the world of similar spiritual traditions, 
especially manifested through archaeology — pyramids, for ex- 
ample. This planet is a theatre of experience in which man has 
the opportunity to grow and learn — as the hermetic texts put it 



1 This is a big canard - as Graham argued that one might have justified the murder of Hitler before the war. Is this not pre- 
ventative warfare? Pre-emptive strikes? This is problematic on theological, philosophical and practical grounds. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 71 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



- to tend the material world — but also to grow at the spiritual 
level — and in order to do so we need to detach from the spiritual 
world. One has to pay attention to the material world, but it is 
not everything. There is a need to earn a living and put food on 
our table. But that is not what we are here to do. We are here to 
perfect the spirit. The ancient Egyptians knew this. They knew 
that we have to grow spiritually. It's not about accumulating 
material growth and many ancient cultures — including Christi- 
anity — knew this. It has a shamanistic element also. We are 
dealing with spiritual and physical realms. Even Christianity 
has this idea. If you look deeply you find a shamanistic trend 
within Christianity — especially with the Gnostics — who to my 
mind were keeping alive an ancient Egyptian tradition. What 
happened was that the bureaucrats and moneymen came in and 
changed Christianity to the notion of obedience to the priests. 
And this brings me back to the Gnostic idea that the priestly 
class — and the church itself — are a demonic force. . . 

If you go back to the Gnostics of the early Christian era 
they have a radical and disturbing view to any person of the 
three Abrahamic faiths: that Tahweh, familiar to Christianity, 
Judaism and Islam, is the Demiurge. He is demonic — evil. He 
has imposed himself and seeks to be worshipped. That's how 
you start to understand the horrific actions of Tahweh. He is a 
brutal being. The Gnostics understood this. But they believed 
that the demiurge was involved in the creation of humanity, and 
that the divine spark is in man. The whole Gnostic project is to 
liberate that spark in man. So the Gnostics turned the garden of 
evil around — the serpent is good - the Tree Of Life can be 
reached - the demiurge wants you not to eat — and this is pro- 
foundly disturbing for Christians. Indeed in the mediaeval times 
it led to horrendous persecution of such a heresy. 

My feeling is that there was a lost civilisation and a spiri- 
tual tradition that taught us to focus on spiritual development. 
This was expressed through metaphors, such as the sacrifice of 
the body as a means of not focusing on the body too much, of 
not being distracted from the spiritual. There was a whole range 
of metaphors. What I think happened as this system devolved 
was that later traditions misinterpreted this, and where sacrifice 
was metaphorical it was taken literal — like the Aztecs — and 
was taken as an instruction to sacrifice bodies. This became 
part of the ritual of human sacrifice being somehow pleasing to 
the gods. This is a perverted interpretation of an ancient teach- 
ing. 

William: Were the Christians autos-da-fe sacrifices 
also? 

Graham: Indeed. The humbug of the Christians! It was 
indeed a sacrifice when someone was burnt at the stake; to save 
the soul through murder. Humbug! I make this point in War 
God. It is no different. 



*** 

The point that Graham raised led me to a question 
that I have presented to my students, and which often 
taxes me. Can we understand, and even forgive, the 
actions of the brutalising colonists and the inquisito- 
rial clerics if we understand their ideological back- 
ground? Can we enter their mind-set and consider 
that they earnestly believed that the Indians were 
damned souls because of their savagery and igno- 
rance, and that therefore if they can convert them to 
Christianity they are rendering them the most blessed 
of gifts, salvation of their eternal souls! Likewise with 
heresy If heresy is a mortal sin, then by torturing the 
heretic you are encouraging him to renounce the 
heresy. You are thus trying to save him. If heresy, fur- 
thermore, is a disease that may be spread amongst 
the populace, then the Inquisitors are acting in the 
public interest by rooting out the heretics, and thus 
saving the people's eternal souls. From our modern 
perspective such a position is unequivocally atrocious, 
and I often observe members of the class horrified by 
the thought that one might even permit a justification for 
this abuse. Can one forgive, or, as many of my stu- 
dents would aver, is it more likely that such persecu- 
tion was merely a theological justification for territo- 
rial land-grab, a smokescreen for empire -building, 
conquest and plunder? 

Graham: Good point. I have no doubt that some of the In- 
quisitors believed that they were doing the right thing that they 
were saving the soul of the individual being consigned to the 
flames. Whilst this excuses nothing it does explain how some- 
one was able to do horrendous acts while believing they were 
doing good things. 

William: And the same could be said of Mocte- 
zuma? 

Graham: Indeed. As I investigate this period it has become 
clear to me that individuals who today we would classify as 
psychopaths were drawn to these roles, as it gave them the justi- 
fication to do what they really loved doing — namely inflicting 
pain and suffering on their fellow man. There are too many 
inquisitors who quite clearly gained pleasure from this. This 
must be taken into account. Secondly we are shaped by cultural 
context. But there is too much of a tendency of our modern 
world to contextualise and to think that it is all a product of 
our conditioning. I disagree with this. We are responsible for our 
actions. 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



72 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



Serendipitously, the interview with Graham ties in 
well with a Masters project I am supervising concern- 
ing cultural interchange between Europe and Mexico 
- trans culturation - focussing on the Christians who 
were desperately trying to bring peace and love and 
good blessing, and who rose up and shouted out 
against the brutality of their countrymen. Not only 
well-known cases like the friars Montesinos, Ber- 
nardino de Sahagiin and Bartolome de Las Casas - 
but also poor mendicant friars who entered villages 
with nothing but a bible, who learnt the indigenous 
languages, who translated sacred texts into and from 
these languages, who saw indigenous people as fel- 
lows. We owe much to these folk for helping to re- 
strain the excesses of conquest and helped to preserve 
some of the indigenous languages, texts, mythologies 
and cosmologies. 

Graham: Indeed. Take, for example, the case of Bartolome 
Olmeda, who urged Cortes not to destroy the idols and not to 
impose Christianity, maintaining that there is no point in forced 
conversion. Cortes was a man with a fanatical streak which 
made him excessive. Consider Las Casas and Sahagiin: these 
guys had been through the cultural conditioning, and yet they 
had the courage to resist it and to say, No! This is wrong. We 
condemn it. We will expose the horror of this. They detested 
what was being done. Amazing that Las Casas was never exe- 
cuted. He seems like a modern human rights activists — what 
courage! It was possible even then to rise above cultural condi- 
tioning. 

We do have choice. This is the issue. We may be con- 
strained by circumstances, but we can always make a choice not 
to inflict suffering on others. Most people in the Sixteenth Cen- 
tury didn't make this choice. They may not have been allowed 
to, but they still could have done so. 

That is the other side of my novels. I talk about Aztec 
wickedness and I talk of Spanish wickedness and I show the 
opposition, like Olmeda. I focus on Munoz in order to show the 
reality of the Inquisition — this horrendous destruction of pre- 
existing knowledge — the codices - the horrible murder of people 
under the pretext that they were heathens. The population of 
Central America plummeted from 30 million to 1 million in 50 
years. This is demonic however you look at it. 

Such a position helps us understand Cortes. It's not so long 
ago that the cultural model of territorial appropriation was still 
present. It is how it still is today. I mean frankly, when a drone 
kills 190 people at a blow, none of whom having performed 
any act of violence against the West, this is sacrifice, murder, 
just like the Aztecs or the Inquisitors. We likewise today have 
our own mechanisms for justifying our actions of aggression. 
Before we condemn those in the past we should look closely at 
ourselves. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



William: Certainly - and one method by which I re- 
late the past with the present in class is to call the Val- 
ladolid Debate of 1550 - in which the churchmen 
Las Casas and Sepiilveda debated the treatment and 
enslavement of the indigenous Americans — an 'en- 
quiry' We are familiar with the Hutton Enquiry 
(which failed to reach any firm resolution on the 
atrocities of the invasion of Iraq). Well, if we take the 
royal debate as being a public view of the horrors of 
a so-called 'just war' then we can see the parallels 
with today. A scrutiny of the underlying notion that 
'our values are right — theirs are wrong' There was 
resistance at the time of Cortes. There is resistance 
today. 

Graham: Right — and at least today we're not burnt at the 
stake for resisting. But still the powerful state apparatus uses 
our money to brutalise people, to impose their will on other cul- 
tures and upon us. The internet is a tool and a forum for resis- 
tance. However, nothing has really changed. The state still 
imposes its will, with the same belief that our system is the 
right one, others are false. This is utter humbug and totally 
disrespectful for other ways of life. And our job in the present 
age is to resist that tooth and nail and to prevent it happening 
whenever we see it. 

William: You have talked recently about the 'reverse 
missionary movement', in which ayahuasceros and 
curanderos of the Amazon are reaching out to the West 
- to the gringos who have traditionally persecuted 
them - in order to spread the word of Mother Aya- 
huasca - in order to instil a vision of nature, peace, 
harmony, and in order to demonstrate to western 
collective consciousness the gradual suicide of the 
human race that is currently taking place under the 
stewardship of western governance. Do you think this 
reverse missionary movement is gathering momen- 
tum? Is the pay-off not immense, in that the gringo 
culture may well appropriate ayahuasca traditions, 
debase them, commercialise them, demonise them, 
etc.? Is the impact of ayahuasca tourism a concern? 

Graham: This is a complex issue. Firstly, the experience of 
contact with an entity or a spirit, that ingestion of ayahuasca 
induces — nobody could fault me for saying that this experience 
is widespread — that people who have drunk ayahuasca all 
around the world experience an entity whom they name Mother 
Ayahuasca. She might be a serpent, which alarms fundamental- 
ist Christians. Sometimes she comes as a beautiful woman. But 
in all cases she comes to teach us something. And those teaching 
are at many levels. People might receive teachings about their 

73 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



own lives, about the mistakes they may be making, and I re- 
cently published an article about this — about how Mother Aya- 
huasca stopped me in my tracks with a 24 year cannabis habit. 
I have nothing against cannabis — nothing at all. It has creative 
and healing properties, and I highly respect the medical mari- 
juana movement. However, there is the question of our individ- 
ual relationship with cannabis. As a 24-year smoker I realised 
that I needed to change, as it brought on paranoia. It made me 
jealous of my wife and partner. And what I was shown in the 
ayahuasca sessions was that all of this was connected to my 
servitude to cannabis. I needed to stop; otherwise I would fall 
into an abyss from which my soul would never recover. All of 
this was shown to me terrijyingly starkly in the ayahuasca vi- 
sions. So much so that when I returned from those sessions in 
Oct 2011 after those five sessions in Brazil, I couldn't smoke 
again. No withdrawal symptoms — no irritability — nothing - 
all gone from my life. I received a teaching in those visions that 
was important to me. Folk who have been addicted to heroin or 
cocaine have reported similar experiences when working with 
ayahuasca. 

And I can't prove anything about the spirit of the vine — 
about any of this — but all I can say is, until you have drunk 
ayahuasca, hold back your judgments. Have a few ceremonies 
and then see how you feel about this entity. And then consider 
that your understanding of reality may not be as you thought - 
and that there may be another realm of reality in which such 
beings do exist. 

Personally, I think that there's an intelligence there, an entity 
who cares for us and for our species. This is what I've felt after 
fifty or so ayahuasca sessions. But I can prove nothing. 

Secondly, in ayahuasca sessions, folk report an intense feel- 
ing of sadness about the environment — particularly about the 
rain forests. Something has gone terribly wrong about the human 
relationship with the planet. The destruction of the forests is a 
dreadful, dreadful mistake that is contrary to what we should be 
here for. A huge opportunity to learn from nature is being taken 
away from us. It has to stop. This is a repeated experience of 
drinkers. 

And so it is an interesting coincidence that just as the Ama- 
zon is being destroyed, this spirit that has always been confined 
to the jungle is moving around the world to deliver these teach- 
ings. 

I feel that there is some intentionality behind this. 

In the Amazon they always talk about the intelligence in the 
vine. How it grabs the leaves in order to gain access to the hu- 
man mind. If there is a goddess or entity, it is pretty clear that 
this entity cannot intervene on this material plain. But it can 
intervene through affecting human consciousness. That's what is 
happening. And people are receiving these teachings, which leads 
to a wish to change the direction of humanity. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



William: Will ayahuasca endure the capitalist urge 
to commercialise and commodify? Is she doing a job 
beyond that which can be sullied? 

Graham: Yes — I feel that the job she is doing will rise above 
this. And, of course, there are people preparing ayahuasca for 
purely commercial gain, people who don't really understand how 
to work with ha. As such I would urge anybody who wants to 
work with ayahuasca to do their research — to use word of 
mouth. A bit like Gnostic sects in the early Christian era - 
underground, illegal, disrespected and discredited. Learn from 
the experiences of others and rely on their anecdotes. 

William: But deeper than that, are there not some 
shamans working for nefarious goals? Something you 
explored in Entangled. And further to that question, 
you recently submitted that: "It would be a good thing I 
couldn't help thinking if every military leader, every religious 
fanatic, every president, every prime minister, every dictator 
presently exercising power in the world were to be required to 
undergo ten sessions of Ayahuasca before being allowed to make 
a single other decision." How, from this perspective, do 
you imagine the great circus of geo-politics would be 
different were such leaders to experience the brew? 
How do we know that if Dick Cheney drank the 
brew, he would not simply gain more power towards 
his power-hungry goals? 

Graham: Good question. Firstly, with the brew, a door into 
another dimension is opened. What's out there isn't only mother 
ayahuasca — there are other entities out there as well. I believe 
in some sense that the duality of good and evil in this realm is 
driven by the good and evil in the spirit realm. And there are 
entities that seem to thrive in a vampire-like way on human 
suffering and misery and on negative human behaviour. 

William: Like Moctezuma and mushrooms 

Graham: Absolutely. The Aztecs went down a dark path 
with mushrooms, opening a channel between Moctezuma and 
Huitzipolochi. 

William: Or Charles Manson and LSD 

Graham: Yes. Some people make these choices in this realm 
and in that realm. People have actively to want to be attracted 
to this negative aspect. Therefore if we gave ayahuasca to a 
power-hungry man could he simply become more powerful? 
Perhaps. But, what can control against that is the set and set- 
ting in which the experience is followed. We call them shamans, 
and our job is to develop a form of shamanism relevant to our 
culture. Some brujos — sorcerers — are doing the opposite of 

74 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



protecting this space, but are opening the door to negative influ- 
ences. I actually believe that were Cheney given a series of ses- 
sions in the right protected space with a skilled and experienced 
shaman — then it is my belief that he would undergo a pro- 
found spiritual transformation. So I believe the teaching needs to 
be handled in a wise and guided way. 

William: Has your relationship with the craft of 
writing changed since engaging in the process of fic- 
tion writing? 

Graham: I'm enjoying writing fiction. Mot so much the 
scenes of violence, but the exercise of not defending every argu- 
ment and writing footnotes for every assertion. It is liberating to 
be free of that. In the case of War God / have done much 
research into the history, and I do refer to that reading. This has 
allowed me to get into the heads of the characters. Once you 
start to get inside the head of Cortes, you start to understand 
things better about these historical characters. The whole his- 
torical canvas has opened up for me in a way like never before. 
The first responsibility as a writer of fiction is not to bore the 
reader. 

William: As for non-fiction 

Graham: Ok, but it is different. There are different argu- 
ments conveyed in non-fiction. Ultimately fiction is about the 
experience more than non-fiction. It's about not delivering every- 
thing at once — about trying to immerse the reader in a visceral 
way in the historical period. It's been an interesting experience 
for me to try to portray these scenes, especially the battles. 

But the second thing is about having something to soy, about 
doing something more than merely entertain the reader. The 
writer must give the readers the sense that they have found some- 
thing out — something of value that they didn't know before. 
That's why I think I will continue fiction, as I can explore 
extraordinary ideas with a freedom that I didn't have before. 
There is of course a need for scholarly criticism — for conserva- 
tism even — as the fire of criticism of new ideas is fundamen- 
tally a good thing. We can't naively accept every thought put out 
there as fact. Indeed, if an idea survives that fire then something 
is in the theory. But, there's a history of throwing the baby out 
with the water with the rejection of ideas. The gate keepers are 
so furious that they look always for what is weak and not look- 
ing for the strengths. 

William: Every new generation needs to start from 
scratch. Myers wrote a century ago in Human Personal- 
ity and Its Survival of Bodily Death: "Now it is that we 
feel the difficulty of being definite without being triv- 
ial; how little of earthly memory persists; how little of 
heavenly experience can be expressed in terms of 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



earth; how long and arduous must be the way, how 
many must be the experiments, and how many the 
failures before any systemised body of new truth can 
be established. But a sound beginning has been 
made, and whatever may be possible hereafter need 
not be wasted on a fresh start." How can we prevent 
constantly having to start again? 

Graham: By considering the knowledge and wisdom of the 
ancients. In 2015 I hope to publish the next part of Finger- 
prints. New theories are coming through which support the 
hypothesis of lost civilisation, that support the dating of an 
immense comet impact in 10,900 BCE — exactly the window 
I suggested in Fingerprints. / suggested this in 1995 but 
there was no evidence. I tried earth-crust displacement and other 
things. This new work on the comet impact is compellingly 
suggestive of the kind of event that could have wiped out a 
civilisation. Then Gobekli Tepe in Turkey was discovered, 
where they are firm that the older layers are over 12,000 years 
ago. This is accepted, but not the implications. What this says 
about human history is not being discussed. Gobekli Tepe is a 
large site of multi-ton megaliths — no background to the culture 
— how come? Something missing in our story — missing in that 
time window when we appear to know that the earth was hit by 
a comet. That missing background intrigues me. I feel it hides 
the lost civilisation. The other interesting thing is carbon- dating. 
Tou can't date stone; so the way it's done is to date organic ma- 
terials associated. This assumption has driven the timeline. The 
problem is that many of these sites are approached not in their 
pristine states. All kinds of things may have happened to these 
sites: many communities may have lived there; the stones may 
have been moved, etc. I've long suggested that some megalithic 
sites are far older than the carbon-dating record shows. There is 
something different at Gobekli Tepe. Work started about 
12,000 years ago then about 10,000 it was deliberately cov- 
ered up. We don't know why. It was hidden and preserved. 
That's why the Germans can be certain of its date. That is 
very important. It raises question marks over other sites - the 
Maltese — the Tala in Menorca — which have the same T 
shape - 1 think it challenges the dating of all megalithic sites. 

William: There are many mysteries that accompany 
any reflection on megalithic sites the world over - 
concerning the carving, the moving, the erecting, and 
the joining, of such immense blocks. The most persis- 
tent explanation of these questions invariably involves 
man-hours - that with enough people over enough 
years such structures as Callanish, Cheops, Tihua- 
naco or Machu Picchu are possible - and there the 
mystery ends. 

However, would you agree that, regardless of the 
mysteries of motives, the significance of the align- 

75 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



ments, and the ritual functions, there are questions 
about the moving and raising of the stones them- 
selves that defy the explanations that have been of- 
fered? Do you feel that there was some technology, 
some harnessing of energy, that was known by mega- 
lithic builders that we neither know nor understand 
today? You illustrate one perspective of this in Entan- 
gled. By what means do you imagine that we may be 
able to recover this technology? 

Graham: / share your intuition. It has been my privilege to 
climb the Great Pyramid jive times. I simply cannot envisage 
man-hours. It's almost magical. The same is true of Sacsay- 
huaman in Peru. It literally beggars belief. I defy man-hours. I 
strongly believe a technology was employed that we don't know 
or understand. We think of mechanical advantage — and as we 
develop there the powers of the mind are diminished. It seems 
that there is a mind-matter influence. And our society have left 
that faculty go dormant. In the past, I think they had great use 
of that; sound, chants, etc. That's one thing I hope to do in the 
sequel to Fingerprints. We've a long way to go. We need to 
overcome our own reference frame. I think the megaliths are 
important. Something important about ourselves that we've 
forgotten about ourselves and that we desperately need to recover. 

*** 

We ended the interview there, although I could hap- 
pily have continued the rap for the rest of the after- 
noon. Off-camera I did manage to ask the one ques- 
tion that I had been itching to ask since reading Su- 
pernatural back in 2005, and which I hadn't managed 
to ask when I spoke to Graham at Breaking Convention: 
what happens on your mushroom trip at Avebury at the end of 
Supernatural? Eager for a story about machine-elves 
cavorting around the megaliths, I received the poetic 
response: 'not much, really' At least one mystery was 
solved. . . 

The mystery of good and evil, however, persists. 
My Libran and liberal constitution urges me to resist 
Graham's invocation to perceive the world in terms 
of good and evil. I strongly wish to see nothing more 
than a vast network of values interacting and conflict- 
ing and generating friction. My willed position is that 
there are no values beyond the context; that forces 
that may be labelled good or evil are constantly in 
opposition and are forever morphing into other forces 
with other values ascribed to them. One can attempt 
to resist evil knowing that evil will never be overcome, 
because there is no such thing. It's just a contextual 
and contingent value. Likewise oppression, aggres- 
sion, violence - there's no genus of these matters - just 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



values that are placed upon acts that just are. Fur- 
thermore, whilst you are resisting 'evil' in one corner 
of the globe, someone else is resisting the 'evil' caused 
by you in another. Systems become oppressive 
through numbers. You resist a system that is only op- 
pressive because it has many people helping it along. 
In fact, you yourself are most likely putting your 
shoulder to the wheel of the very system that you are 
now trying to resist. Then you realise there is no sys- 
tem - there are just endless interacting processes that 
themselves are endlessly interacting processes. Push 
somewhere and something will move somewhere else. 
This is the position that I try to maintain, yet I under- 
stand that such a position can become a nominalist 
chaos. Graham makes this clear - dualism is a useful 
teaching tool. Sitting on the fence is not effective re- 
sistance. 

I reflected on why the presentation of violence 
and brutality in Entangled and War God was of such 
importance to me. I came to realise that I was critical 
- disdainful even - of the depiction of sacrifice, tor- 
ture, sadism, warfare and bloodshed in both novels. 
Was Graham not dwelling on these horrors to an 
unwholesome degree? Is there not something unsa- 
voury about such graphic images? 'Absolutely' was 
his pragmatic answer, 'I am dwelling on these matters 
to an unwholesome degree, because we cannot pre- 
tend that this savagery has not, and does not, take 
place.' This is central to the vision that Graham de- 
scribes being given with ayahuasca. We cannot ignore 
the darkness. 

I then understood that Graham could easily have 
come back to me 'but you do the same!', and with 
good reason, as I have run an annual open lecture at 
the university to which I have invited speakers, such 
as human rights lawyers and journalists and even a 
former captive at Guantanamo, to demonstrate to the 
audience the horrors that are committed by our gov- 
ernments in our name. Were someone critically to say 
to me, 'oh, you shouldn't be doing that. Why dwell on 
torture and incarceration?' I would reply, 'on the con- 
trary, I don't do it enough — not enough at all, as I shy 
away from such matters as soon as I can under the 
justification that I shouldn't fill my life with darkness.' 
The suffering of torture in captivity is no different 
now than it was at the time of Cortes. That is the 
point, regardless of its name, whether 'evil' or 'bad' 
or 'nasty' or 'dark.' If you are tortured, or witness the 
murder of your friends and family, or hear the sound 
of gunfire meant for you - you are not going to smile 
calmly and say - 'I accept this as a manifestation of 
the Unified Good.' You are not going to confront 

76 



Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



your torturer or the killer of your children with 'Hey 
there! I don't really believe that you're evil — in fact I 
love you. . .' We may all aspire to such mystic bliss, but 
it is unlikely to save you. It will not stop them hurting 
you, and it will not stop it hurting. 

That, I understood, is precisely the opposition that 
is developed in Graham's novels. The characters 
learn to love and trust, and in this way they oppose 
hate. Love is naturally loving. Hate is naturally hating. 
Ria and Leoni resist the terrible Sulpa in Entangled 
not by loving him, but by loving and trusting and 
helping each other. Tozi, Malinal and Pepillo in War 
God likewise oppose the hatred of their oppressors not 
by loving them, but by loving each other. There is a 
pragmatic, battle-weary realistic aspect to these con- 
flicts. 

Sulpa, Huitzilopochtli and St Peter are discarnate 
entities inflicting their sordid lust for conflict and 
desolation upon humans; 'That's what demons do' 
Graham declared, 'multiply human misery' Is there 
something chilling in this depiction of the power of 
malevolent demons, especially given that Graham 
experienced visions of these forces in ayahuasca en- 
counters? Again, Graham's pragmatic and valuable 
response is that just as we cannot turn our backs on 
human cruelty, neither can we assume that all intelli- 
gent entities of the spiritual world are benign. On the 
contrary, as he explained so thoroughly, shamans and 
curanderos need to be particularly adept at navigating 
these otherworld landscapes and deflecting the influ- 
ence of mischievous or downright nasty entities in 
order to open the healing space and prevent pain and 
suffering. Above all they need to be resolute enough 
to resist the lure of power that emanates from these 
more malign beings. Entangled depicts this struggle 
through a brujo who derives power to do harm and 
the shamans who need confront these brujos in the 
visionary realm. This is real. This is how it is, and 
nobody who has drunk ayahuasca, or attended any 
meeting in which non-material beings are contacted, 
or read Swedenborg, would be naive enough to sup- 
pose that all there is angelic and benign. 

Graham's readers will naturally be drawn into 
speculation about major historical conflicts and the 
invocation of these dark entities. This is a weird and 
unsettling area of exploration, as the speculation can 
escalate into a feeling that such conflicts are created 
and controlled by non-material intelligences. It need 
not be so dramatic. If, as anyone sympathetic to the 
matter would aver, there are further dimensions of 
reality populated by discarnate souls of the dead and 
non-human entities (something Graham explores lu- 

Vol. 4 No. 3 



cidly in Supernatural), then it is perfectly appropriate to 
assume that human participants in such worldly con- 
flicts may have derived power or knowledge from 
these entities. Shamans, brujos and curanderos do so on 
a daily basis in more localised conflicts. The relation- 
ships between Cortes and St Peter or between Moc- 
tezuma and Huitzilopochtli are thus far less outland- 
ish than may be assumed. They are part of the cur- 
rency of reality only on a vast scale. The burning 
question is not whether this occurs, but according to 
which ideologies. If Christian world leaders invoke 
God prior to launching a campaign of aggression 
against Islam; if a Moslem invokes Allah prior to re- 
taliating with equally indiscriminate hostilities; or if 
Jewish settlers invoke their God prior to clearing a 
land of its non-Jewish occupants, are they invoking 
spiritual forces of compassion and love, or of hatred 
and violence? To whom are they really praying? 

This, of course, is the link with Graham's work 
Talisman, which looks at the troubled history of eso- 
teric gnostic thought which has stealthily crept 
through the alleys and byways of history, occasionally 
blossoming in moments like Alexandria in the early 
Christian Roman period, the Cathar era of southern 
France and the Pyrenees, and the Florentine Renais- 
sance. The message that Graham takes to be central 
of this ancient lineage of heretical thought is that the 
Christian god is not the prime creator, but a lesser 
entity - the Demiurge - who demands worship and 
obedience and instils hatred and fear. The structure 
of the church is thus a political institution in the serv- 
ice of this malevolent supernatural despot. The hor- 
ror of European colonial conquest, the Inquisition, 
the brutal suppression of heresy such as the Albigen- 
sian Crusade, and endless wars of aggression can 
thus be understood as influenced by the presence of 
this demiurgic force. Graham, aware that this posi- 
tion may well offend, has discussed this in many pres- 
entations and interviews over the last few years, and 
one can see its integral relationship with the narra- 
tives of his two novels. 

Hans Jonas describes in The Gnostic Religion the 
turbulent landscape out of which diverse religious- 
spiritual schools of gnostic thought arose especially in 
the Hellenised parts of the Roman Empire in the first 
centuries after Christ. This was a period of such cul- 
tural upheaval, of such calamities and conflicts, and 
of such an eradication of earlier spiritual models, 
that inevitably many folk looked upon their troubled 
landscapes and questioned whether this could really 
be the work of a great, ineffable and perfect god. It 
was reasonable to assume that it was not. As Graham 

77 



Paeanthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



and Robert Bauval explore in Talisman, there was also 
the attempt amongst these gnostic communities to 
keep alive more ancient, pantheistic, mystical and 
perhaps harmonious spiritual teachings. This, argues 
Graham, is the parallel with the modern world, 
where destruction of the natural world and of the 
biosphere has reached such proportions that it would 
appear that human life itself is threatened; where 
corporatism and imperialism proliferate violence and 
enslavement and where individual human gnosis is 
declared anathema. The gnostic heresy is a wisdom 
that teaches people to question these brutalising insti- 
tutions, to challenge their hegemony over the values 
of individuals and communities, and to experience 
the mysteries of reality first hand, joyfully. This is the 
wisdom that is always persecuted by the institutions, 
whether Church or State, or, as is so often the case, 
the Church-State brotherhood. Interestingly, Graham 
sees the gnosis of ayahuasca traditions and experi- 
ences as being analogous to the ancient gnostic tradi- 
tions in the West. 

One need not accept as gospel the dualistic, 
Manichean, view of good and evil in order to recog- 
nise the powerful spiritual wisdom in these gnostic 
traditions. One of Terence McKenna's more beauti- 
ful and melancholic presentations is Unfolding the Stone, 
in which, like Hans Jonas, he examines the traditions 
of alchemy beginning in the unsettled era of the 
early Christian years of the late Roman Empire. The 
vision of good and evil was of less importance to 
McKenna than were the twin radical gnostic teach- 
ings which rocked both the Greco-Hellenic world and 
the Roman-Christian world: that we are all divine, 
luminous, beings; and that fate can be overcome 
through magic. This, again, is the connection that is 
made both by Terence and by Graham between ex- 
panded consciousness achieved through psychedelics 
and gnostic esotericism. This is the powerful narra- 
tive of both of Graham's novels. Evil will not be 
overcome, as it is a force beyond the control of man- 
kind, but it can be resisted. Fate can be overcome 
simply because we can choose our paths. We are not 
bound to consent to the control over our sovereignty 
that state and state religion demand. The further we 
explore the immeasurable capacity of our conscious- 
ness, whether with the assistance of psychedelics or 
through other empowering practices, the more we 
can understand how political structures can belittle 
and ridicule such capacities. This is what Ria and 
Tozi understand. This is what Leoni and Pepillo un- 
derstand. We can overcome fate through the magic of 
resistance. 

Vol. 4 No. 3 




I write this while the press reveals the full extent of 
state secret surveillance, while the Bilderberg Group 
meets in secrecy to draft global policy, and while Brit- 
ish Foreign Secretary William Hague delivers the poi- 
soned words 'if you've done nothing wrong you've 
nothing to be afraid of How arbitrary might the 
state's judgment of right and wrong be. How easily 
might one be branded a heretic. How readily do we 
relinquish power for security. How fickly is this secu- 
rity. How gleefully do we surrender our power to cor- 
porations, interacting socially through mechanisms 
that so easily become systems of surveillance. How 
gladly do we accept that we are not divine beings. 
How urgent is the current appeal to resist. . . 

Biographies 

William Rowlandson is a 

Senior Lecturer in Hispanic 
Studies at the University of 
Kent, and former Director of 
the Centre for the Study of 
Myth. He has recently com- 
pleted a book concerning 
Borges and mysticism, which 
examines the relationship 
between Borges' own re- 
corded mystical experiences 
and his appraisal of Sweden- 
borg and other mystics. The book asks the essential ques- 
tion of whether Borges was a mystic by analysing his writ- 
ings, including short stories, essays, poems and interviews, 
alongside scholarly writings on mysticism by figures such 
as William James. William's work within the Myth Centre 
has focused predominantly on the many aspects of the 
work of Jung. With co-Director Angela Voss, William organ- 
ised a conference at the University of Kent in May 2011 
entitled Daimonic Imagination: Uncanny Intelligence. 

Graham Hancock is the author of 
The Sign and The Seal, Fingerprints 
of the Gods, Keeper Of Genesis, 
Heaven's Mirror, and other bestsell- 
ing investigations of historical mys- 
teries. His recent work focuses on 
shamanism and the origins of relig- 
ion. His 2005 book, Supernatural: 
Meetings with The Ancient Teachers 
of Mankind, suggests that experi- 
ences in altered states of con- 
sciousness have played a funda- 
mental role in the evolution of hu- 
man culture and that other realities 
not normally accessible to our senses may surround us at 
all times. While researching Supernatural. His experiences 
with the ayahuasca lead to his first novel, Entangled. His 
latest book War God was published earlier this year. His 
books have sold more than five million copies worldwide 
and have been translated into twenty-seven languages. 



78 




Paranthropolocy: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



[REVIEW] Through a Glass Darkly: 
Magic, Dreams & Prophecy in Ancient Egypt 

Callum E. Cooper 



Through a Glass 
Darkly 

Magic, dreams and prophecy 
in ancient Egypt 




Edit eA by 

Kasia Szpakowska 



laranthropology Journal couldn't be a more apt place to 
briefly discuss the following book, due to the interre- 
lated issues of ancient human experiences, mysticism 
and anomalous phenomena. It is not the first time 
ancient Egypt and anomalous experiences have been 
addressed in this journal (Cooper 2011). And though 
it is rare to find literature that exclusively addresses 
the subject of anomalous experiences in Egypt, there 
have been some classic overviews of the subject pro- 
duced in rare pieces (e.g., Dingwall 1930). 

Through a Glass Darkly is a compilation of papers 
from a 2003 conference highlighting current research 
and findings on phenomena related to magic, 
dreams, and prophecy in Ancient Egypt. The presen- 
tations were given by several well noted Egyptologists 
at the Baskerville Hall in Wales (near the village of 
Hay-on-Wye), UK. Half of the papers presented of- 
fer new theories and discussions of already well- 
established knowledge on ancient Egyptian views on 
dreams and their meaning, as well as on magic and 
prophecy. While other papers consider modern re- 
search, such as Robert Ritner, who presents new data 
on a bronze serpent wand, which is frequently found 
in Egyptian iconography. 

The most intriguing papers, particularly where 
paranthropology is concerned, are those addressing 
the ancient views and interpretations of dreams and 
magic. In the literature available on ancient Egypt 
and dreams, particularly with Dr Szpakowska's work, 
and Dingwall (1930), dreams were seen as a gateway 
to the future, they could deliver precognitive mes- 
sages or inspiration from supreme beings (much like 
apparitions (Dodds, 1971)). Dreams and precogni- 
tions have been found to be a natural aspect of hu- 
man nature, from ancient times to the present day. 
Research on the experiences of the ancient Egyptians 
and contemporary anomalous phenomena has begun 
to show that paranormal phenomena seem to be 
natural and unchanged in their characteristics 
throughout time (see Cooper, 2012). However, the 
papers presented in Through a Glass Darkly also dem- 



Through a Glass Darkly: Magic, Dreams and 
Prophecy in Ancient Egypt. 
Edited by Kasia Szpakowska 
(The Classical Press of Wales, Hardback, 2006) 
RRP £45.00 

onstrate that such experiences are influenced by re- 
ligious interpretation. Much relation is given to the 
ancient gods of the time and their involvement in 
dreams, magic and prophecy. 

Conferences such as this, and the production of 
presented papers in the form of a book, are highly 



Vol. 4 No. 3 



79 



Paranthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 



useful in updating the information and knowledge we 
have on history and ancient societies (or any issues of 
the arts and sciences). Though much like social sci- 
ence, ancient history will always discover something 
knew, and by understanding what we know about 
human-beings from the social sciences, it helps us to 
understand ancient history and our development 
throughout time. In this instance, an understanding 
of the paranthropological aspects of human nature 
works well with the topics at hand, presented by Dr 
Szpakowska. 

Dr Szpakowska made the excellent point that 
academic study for a long time regarding magic, the 
mystical (especially the paranormal), has for a long 
time been misunderstood and shunned. Conferences, 
papers, books and educational programs are increas- 
ing, regarding such teachings on the transpersonal, 
mystical and paranormal. Even dreams within psy- 
chology, from its Freudian beginning and onwards, 
produced much criticisms as to whether pursuit of 
studying such human features in modern day and 
ancient history can teach us more about human so- 
cieties. Dr Szpakowska (2003) has demonstrated oth- 
erwise, and publication on such matters has increased 
greatly in the last decade or so - especially with re- 
gards to the interpretation of dreams in ancient 
Egypt. 

Through a Glass Darkly is a highly thought out col- 
lection of Egyptological papers which I believe would 
interest most people involved in paranthropology and 
parapsychology - aside from Egyptology. As with 
anomalous experiences, ancient societies and their 
history present a puzzle for researchers, which re- 
quire the gaps to be filled to help us learn more, 
rather than simply making assumptions about how 
things in the past or present work and are inter- 
preted. This is why all research in social sciences has 
its importance in our understanding of human func- 
tioning in life, and our actions towards others. It is 
only our personal assumptions and biases which af- 
fect our understanding of ancient societies, and how 
human systems work, rather than learning from 
available evidence, as Dr Szpakowska points out. 

It is further stated that anthropologists have the 
advantage of living samples to learn from, while the 
Egyptologist does not, and they must draw informa- 
tion about the dead together, to form an understand- 
ing. However, as we know from paranthropology, the 
line between anthropology and Egyptology is argua- 
bly level. The book itself hopes to inspire new think- 
ing in - what I would class as - the paranthropological 



aspects, surrounding practices and beliefs in magic, 
dreams and prophecy in ancient societies. In quote: 

In the end, perhaps the real beauty of the study of 
ancient civilizations such as Ancient Egypt, is that 
we can pursue the vapour trails left by individuals 
in the past, the data and ideas of yesterday, and re- 
examine and reconfigure them in tomorrow's new 
light (Szpakowska, 2006, p.xiv) 

I believe researchers dealing with any forms of excep- 
tional human experiences can learn from this state- 
ment. And, to any researchers of such phenomena, 
this book is certainly of use in understanding experi- 
ences of the ancient societies; in order to greater un- 
derstand the experiences of today. 

References: 

Cooper, C.E. (201 1). The Ka of ancient Egypt. Par- 
anthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to 
the Paranormal, 2(3), 43-45. 

Cooper, C. E. (2012). Social views on apparitions and sur- 
vival in ancient and modern Egypt. Paper presented 
at the 36 th International Conference of the Soci- 
ety for Psychical Research, University of North- 
ampton, UK. 

Dingwall, E.J. (1930). Ghosts and spirits in the ancient 
world. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & 
Co. 

Dodds, E.R. (1971). Supernormal phenomena in 
classical antiquity. Proceedings of the Society for Psy- 
chical Research, 55, 189-237. 

Szpakowska, K. (2003). Behind closed eyes: Dreams and 
nightmares in Ancient Egypt. Swansea: The Classi- 
cal Press of Wales. 



Callum E. Cooper, University of 
Northampton, Centre for the Study of 
Anomalous Psychological Processes 

callum.cooper@northampton.ac.uk 




Vol. 4 No. 3 



80 



Parcmthropology: 
Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal 

We live in a world that is impossibly more fantastic than the present 
materialist and scientistic paradigms allow. In such a gross mis- 
match between the weirddom of the real and the Flatland of the 
boring and banal, it is so hopeful, and so refreshing, to see serious 
intellectuals take the strange so seriously. What we have with this 
new journal and this remarkable collection of essays is a cause for 

celebration. 

Dr. Jeffrey J. Kripal, Author of Authors of the Impossible: The Sa- 
cred and the Paranormal. 

In 1908 William James wrote that: 'The great world, the back- 
ground, in all of us, is the world of our beliefs. That is the world of 
the permanencies and the immensities.' More than a century later 
Jack Hunter has collated a selection of thought-provoking new nar- 
ratives to help chart the geography of this world. In essays cover- 
ing ground all the way from Tibet to Taiwan and into the landscape 
of dreams and the Afterlife an array of internationally renowned 
researchers discuss the discourse between the human, natural and 
supernatural worlds in a bold attempt to record the history and 
current affairs of this as yet mostly undiscovered country of which 
we all (like it are not) are citizens. 

Dr. Wendy Cousins, University of Ulster. 

In a short period of time, Paranthropology has established itself as a 
serious and intelligent voice in the difficult and sensitive area of the anthropological study of the 'paranormal.' We 
are living in a complicated period in relation to our understanding of 'extraordinary' phenomena. Naive materialist 
approaches are more assertive than ever, in anthropology and in the world more generally. At the same time, the 
taboos against admitting to the reality of the paranormal are weakening. There is a growing body of writing which 
takes the paranormal and extraordinary seriously, while bringing to it the same academic standards that any other 
subject matter would require. This is a valuable and important development, and it helps open the way to new modes 
of understanding in the sciences and social sciences that will not reject scientific rationality, but expand that rational- 
ity so as to include more of the world of human experience. The articles in this Paranthropology reader provide im- 
portant clues and suggestions, along with rigorous argument, to help us in exploring what is likely to be a major 

area of anthropological engagement in coming years. 

Dr. Geoffrey Samuel, Research Group on the Body, Health and Religion (BAHAR), School of History, Archaeology 

and Religion, Cardiff University. 

This compilation is an impressive collection of academic approaches to the anthropological study of anomalous 
experience. ..To have a collection of essays of this calibre in one volume makes this book a real gem. Previously those 
of us interested in the anthropological approach to the paranormal had to make do with occasional contributions in 

other publications. 

David Taylor, Anomaly: Journal of Research Into the Paranormal 




http://paranthropologyjournal.weebly.com/anthology.html 




www.paranthropology.co.uk